Key assessment reading logs and powerpoint
First, read the provided articles. You will read two articles per issue, making a total of 12 articles. For each article, write a reading log summary, reaction, and three questions.
The reading logs are to be done in a Microsoft office word doc.
There should be a total of 16 slides for the power point not including cover page and reference page.
Create a
PowerPoint presentation and submit
12 reading logs where you describe and discuss the following key elements of infant development.
Key Elements of Infant Development
1. Infant Feeding – Is it best to breast feed or bottle feed?
2. Infant Feeding – Is it best to feed on a schedule or “on demand?”
3. Infant Sleeping – Is it best for a baby to sleep alone in own room or to co-sleep in same bed with parents?
4. Infant Crying – Is it best to pick baby up when baby cries or to let baby cry it out? (Infant crying refers to more than just crying at bedtime. Infant crying refers to any time an infant cry.)
5. Continuity of Care – What is continuity of care and what are the pros and cons? (The continuity of care term for this course refers to the practice of keeping infants and teachers together for more than one year.)
6. Attachment Theory – What is attachment theory and why is it important?
First, read the provided articles. You will read two articles per issue, making a total of 12 articles. For each article, write a reading log summary, reaction, and three questions. You will submit these twelve reading logs along with your PowerPoint
Next, create a PowerPoint presentation. Create three slides for each issue, except for issue number 6. For issue number 6, only one slide is needed. For issues, 1 – 5, on the first slide, list main points for the first side of the issue (for example, breast feeding). On the second slide, list main points for the other side of the issue (for example, bottle feeding). On the third slide state your opinion. When you create your slides, you should use bullet points for the slides. You should not have more than 6 bullet points per slide and not more than 4 or 5 words after each bullet point. You may use the notes section of the PowerPoint to provide detailed explanations written in complete sentences.
Finally, create a final slide in your PowerPoint where you discuss how early childhood programs influence an infant’s development and what the short and long term effects of an early childhood program might be on an infant’s development. Refer to academic articles in this discussion. Feel free to include pictures or drawings to enhance your discussion.
Summary of Infant responsive feeding article:
In this article the authors use the term responsive feeding to mean feeding on demand. They suggest that if mothers do not use on demand feeding their babies will become overweight. The idea is that babies need to learn what it feels like to feel hungry. When they feel hungry they need to call out to their mothers and have their mothers respond right away by feeding them. If the mothers listen to their babies’ cues (crying) and feed them, the babies will develop self-regulation and self-control because they will learn that when they have a need the need will be met. They will learn to eat when they are hungry. Babies feel confident and secure when they signal that a need (hunger) needs to be met and then the need is actually met right then and there. If mothers try to feed their babies when they are not hungry, the babies will learn to eat when someone else thinks they should eat or just because it is a certain time, which could lead to weight problems later in life. Healthy eating is when a baby eats when she is hungry not when it is a certain time or when someone else thinks she should eat.
Skip To Content
University of Notre Dame
Mother-Baby Behavioral Sleep Laboratory
Guidelines to Sleeping Safe with Infants:
Adapted from: Maximizing the chances of Safe Infant Sleep in the
Solitary and Cosleeping (Specifically, Bed-sharing) Contexts,
by
James J. McKenna, Ph.D. Professor of Biological Anthropology, Director,
Mother-Baby Sleep Laboratory, University of Notre Dame.
Below is a summary that highlights some of the issues to be concerned
with as you make your own decisions about where and how your infant
should sleep.
What constitutes a “safe sleep environment” irrespective of where the infant sleeps?
Safe infant sleep ultimately begins with a healthy gestation.
Specifically, safe infant sleep begins without the fetus being exposed
to maternal smoke during pregnancy.
A second factor that has a strong influence on safe infant sleep is
breastfeeding. Breastfeeding significantly helps to protect infants from
death including deaths from SIDS/SUDI and from secondary disease and/or
congenital conditions. Post-natally safe infant sleep begins especially
with the presence of an informed, breastfeeding, committed mother, or
an informed and committed father.
Regardless of whether an infant sleeps on the same surface as his or
her parents, on a same-surface co-sleeper, in a bassinet or in a
separate crib, in the same room as their parents or in a separate room,
all infants should follow these same guidelines: infants
should always sleep on their backs, on firm surfaces, on clean surfaces,
in the absence of (secondhand) smoke, under light (comfortable)
blanketing, and their heads should never be covered.
The bed should not have any stuffed animals or pillows around the
infant and never should an infant be placed to sleep on top of a pillow
or otherwise soft bedding.
Sheepskins or other fluffy material and especially beanbag mattresses
should never be used with infants. Waterbeds can be especially
dangerous to infants too, and no matter the type of mattress, it should
always tightly intersect the bed-frame to leave no gaps or space.
Infants should never sleep on couches or sofas with or without adults as
they can slip down (face first) into the crevice or get wedged against
the back of a couch where they may suffocate.
Bedsharing: It is important to be aware that adult beds were not designed to assure infants safety!
Aside from never letting an infant sleep outside the presence
of a committed adult, i.e. separate-surface cosleeping which is safe
for all infants, I do not recommend to any parents any particular
type of sleeping arrangement since I do not know the circumstances
within which particular parents live. What I do recommend is to consider
all of the possible choices and to become as informed as is possible
matching what you learn with what you think can work the best for you
and your family.
Copyright
© 2022
University of Notre Dame
Mother-Baby Behavioral Sleep Laboratory
Share
Department of Anthropology, University of Notre Dame Notre Dame, IN 46556
e:
james.j.mckenna.25@nd.edu
Contact
Accessibility Information
Schedule or on demand? (Feeding schedules for babies)
Chatelaine
, English edition
68.5
(May 1995): 36.
1.
Full text
2.
Abstract
/Details
Turn on hit highlighting for speaking browsers by selecting the Enter button
Abstract
Translate [unavailable for this document]
Like many first-time mothers, I buried myself in baby books. I knew yesterday’s rigid feeding schedules were passe. Happy babies feed on demand, said experts such as Penelope Leach.
That’s true in the first few months, when a newborn’s small stomach cannot hold much milk or formula (nursing infants need to feed every couple of hours). But by around three or four months, round-the-clock feedings are hard on parents, especially if they have other children, and may not be in the baby’s best interests. Ruth McCamus, a nurse at The Hospital for Sick Children’s Centre for Health Information and Promotion in Toronto, is a great believer in routine. “It helps to plant the idea in the baby’s mind that there is a regularity to the way things happen.”
Like many first-time mothers, I buried myself in baby books. I knew yesterday’s rigid feeding schedules were passe. Happy babies feed on demand, said experts such as Penelope Leach.
That’s true in the first few months, when a newborn’s small stomach cannot hold much milk or formula (nursing infants need to feed every couple of hours). But by around three or four months, round-the-clock feedings are hard on parents, especially if they have other children, and may not be in the baby’s best interests. Ruth McCamus, a nurse at The Hospital for Sick Children’s Centre for Health Information and Promotion in Toronto, is a great believer in routine. “It helps to plant the idea in the baby’s mind that there is a regularity to the way things happen.”
You have requested “on-the-fly” machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation.
Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated “AS IS” and “AS AVAILABLE” and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from.
Hide full disclaimer
Translations powered by LEC.
Translations powered by LEC.
Full Text
Translate [unavailable for this document]
When my daughter was born nearly two years ago, I was prepared for the worst–feedings every two hours, months of sleepless nights. But I was lucky. From birth, Lucy knew night from day and–with the exception of a brief burst of nursing enthusiasm in the evenings–fed only every three or four hours.
Like many first-time mothers, I buried myself in baby books. I knew yesterday’s rigid feeding schedules were passe. Happy babies feed on demand, said experts such as Penelope Leach.
That’s true in the first few months, when a newborn’s small stomach cannot hold much milk or formula (nursing infants need to feed every couple of hours). But by around three or four months, round-the-clock feedings are hard on parents, especially if they have other children, and may not be in the baby’s best interests. Ruth McCamus, a nurse at The Hospital for Sick Children’s Centre for Health Information and Promotion in Toronto, is a great believer in routine. “It helps to plant the idea in the baby’s mind that there is a regularity to the way things happen.”
Routine tends to become an issue when a mother is returning to work. Day-care workers in particular often cannot cope easily with an infant who expects to feed when the spirit moves him.
If your baby has not already established a pattern by age four months–that is, you know roughly what to expect from one day to the next–then you’re wise to start encouraging one, McCamus says. But regular doesn’t necessarily mean rigid: the goal is a flexible routine that works for both of you.
Feeding without fuss
If your baby feeds well and then cries two hours later, try to postpone the breast or bottle for a while by playing with her or having a cuddle. If she’s a snacker who doesn’t feed long enough to fill up, take a break for 15 or 20 minutes. Then, offer more milk or formula.
If your baby has day/night confusion, wake him up during the day for regular feedings and playtimes every two to three hours. Keep late-night mealtimes quiet and dimly lit so as not to wake him completely. Eventually, he’ll get the message.
Don’t wake a sleeping baby in the middle of the night for a feeding unless there is a medical reason to do so, such as insufficient weight gain.
Don’t be afraid to give a feeding a little earlier than usual if your baby is clearly hungry. Similarly, if she’s content upon waking, immediate feeding may not be necessary.
Trust your instincts. Too much reliance on parenting books can cause confusion.
Word count:
435
Show less
You have requested “on-the-fly” machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation.
Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated “AS IS” and “AS AVAILABLE” and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from.
Hide full disclaimer Translations powered by LEC.
Translations powered by LEC.
Copyright Rogers Publishing Limited May 1995
image2.gif
image1.gif
_______________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________
Report Information from ProQuest
April 07 2016 14:35
_______________________________________________________________
07 April 2016 ProQuest
Table of contents
1.
………………………………………………………………………………………….. 1
07 April 2016 ii ProQuest
Document 1 of 1
How to get a grip on attachment theory
Author: Townshend, Kate
ProQuest document link
Abstract: […]I wonder how many of us are really clear about what attachment theory is. “The preliminary
evidence from our pilot study shows that attachment-based practice in schools can have a positive impact on
academic performance, reduction in behavioural incidents and improvements in pupil and staff well-being.”
Full text: Behaviour issues caused by trauma are more common than you may think – but a new project is
developing strategies to help
Fresh from teacher training and on the supply circuit, I spent some time in a Reception classroom where I
discovered that size is no predictor of the severity of behaviour problems. Faced with a tiny boy who spat,
shouted and, at the age of 5, knew more swear words than I did, I was baffled, panicked and eventually angry.
Then, at lunchtime, his personal teaching assistant returned from training and my miniature Mr Hyde was
suddenly transformed into an altogether calmer Dr Jekyll, visibly relaxing after a hug and some soothing words
from the saintly Mrs M. At the time, I assumed his poor behaviour was entirely down to something I had done
wrong. But I later learned that this student was suffering from attachment issues.
Dr Maggie Atkinson, the children’s commissioner for England, believes that “every teacher in every school”
should be aware of attachment theory and tailor their practice accordingly. But I wonder how many of us do. In
fact, I wonder how many of us are really clear about what attachment theory is. I certainly wasn’t able to spot
the signs when they were right in front of me.
Be aware of the basics
British psychologist John Bowlby first articulated attachment theory (in the terms we now understand it) in the
mid-20th century. It is an attempt to explain how the relationship between a child and its parents, particularly the
mother, influences development.
Bowlby believed that in the early years of life, a child will form an attachment with a single primary care-giving
figure and that this relationship will be a prototype for all future relationships. He argued that should an
attachment fail to form during this period, or be disrupted, then a number of consequences would follow,
including behaviour problems and reduced ability to learn.
Teachers should be aware of the implications of this theory. But how prevalent is the problem and how do you
identify it? The majority of parent-child relationships appear to be strong, after all.
Fortunately, identifying children with attachment issues is possible. Schools may already be aware of
circumstances that could affect attachment. If not, resilience is often a key factor: a child who gives up at the
first sign of failure may be lacking the secure base required to try again.
And more children may be suffering from these issues than you expect. A key study on attachment, conducted
by Christi and David Bergin in 2009, estimates that up to a third of all children have an insecure attachment to at
least one caregiver. A 2004 study from Scotland, meanwhile, posits that almost all children will have
experienced trauma of some kind by the end of their primary years.
But just being aware of attachment theory is not enough: teachers need to use it to inform how they teach and
form relationships with students.
So where to begin? The Attachment Aware Schools project brings together university-based researchers and
local practitioners in Somerset to provide training on bringing ideas about attachment into classrooms (for more
information, visit www.attachmentawareschools.com).
In 2013 and 2014, the project’s organisers ran a pilot study in 11 schools and colleges, with each committing to
a programme of training in areas such as trauma and its implications for learning. Each institution devised its
07 April 2016 Page 1 of 4 ProQuest
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1625312946?accountid=11330
own whole-school approaches to being “attachment aware” with the help of a consultant.
The initial results were excellent, with all participants reporting that they had found the process useful and that it
had had a big impact on learning.
Dr Janet Rose of Bath Spa University is part of the research group leading the programme. She explains:
“Attachment theory is already well recognised in areas such as clinical psychology, health and social care.
“The preliminary evidence from our pilot study shows that attachment-based practice in schools can have a
positive impact on academic performance, reduction in behavioural incidents and improvements in pupil and
staff well-being.”
Putting theory into practice
So far, so good. But what does attachment theory look like in practice? And what new strategies are emerging?
One example is the use of “emotion coaching”. Originating in the US and historically used in helping parents to
relate to their children, it involves recognising, naming and validating the feelings and emotions that may cause
disruptive behaviour rather than dismissing them.
In particular, there is an emphasis on changing the language used to deal with poor behaviour, enabling
children to understand and manage their feelings more successfully.
For example, parents would be advised to say: “I’m sorry you’re feeling so angry and I’d like to talk to you more
about it, but I still need you to stop throwing things” (instead of: “It’s very silly to throw things so stop it right
now!”). It is a strategy that has been embraced by many of the pilot schools in the Attachment Aware scheme,
and Ed Harker, headteacher of St Saviours CEVC Nursery and Infant School in Bath, has already seen positive
results.
“We have found that children with attachment disorders have responded really well,” he explains. “And on a
practical level, attachment theory has changed the specific language we use with all the children.”
The Attachment Aware project and its pilot schools offer further specific ways of bringing attachment into an
educational setting, from appointing an attachment leader to using nurture groups to help children process their
feelings better.
Harker says that the interventions at St Saviours – such as a “nurture” space for children to use at lunchtime –
have had a school-wide impact.
“There is a better emotional intelligence within the school team now and a raised awareness of attachment
needs,” he says. “It gives you the understanding you need to adapt your provision and ensure that potentially
vulnerable children are helped to thrive.”
Fighting the fear
But it is not just about interventions, according to Dr Rose. What can also be effective in schools, she believes,
is the modelling of attachment. “Most teachers are likely to encounter insecurely attached and traumatised
children in their classrooms,” she says. “A warm, supportive teacher can actually promote attachment
relationships.”
Roger Catchpole, a consultant for children’s mental health charity YoungMinds, agrees: “There is strong
evidence for the protective power of one important adult in a child’s life who can provide them with unconditional
positive regard. This is often found in school.” He points out that this does not have to be a teacher and could
equally be a teaching assistant or other member of support staff.
The Attachment Aware project also recommends assigning “key adults” to needy children. These are trained
adults within the school who take a particular interest in a child, supporting their emotional development and
reducing their anxiety. And although “unconditional positive regard” may seem to suggest overlooking or
indulging poor behaviour, it is more about giving children a sense that they are liked and cared for in spite of
their behaviour.
This may make some teachers feel uncomfortable, particularly considering that adults are often advised to keep
a careful distance from students for child protection and authority. But as Harker says, if attachments are
07 April 2016 Page 2 of 4 ProQuest
“appropriate” and “secure” there is nothing to fear.
Fear is perhaps as important a factor as ignorance in stopping attachment theory being more widely
implemented in schools. We have become so guarded when it comes to children and so concerned with data
that we can forget about the importance of relationships in teaching. For those children lucky enough to get a
great start in life, this is damaging. But for those with attachment issues it can be nothing short of destructive.
Kate Townshend is a teacher at a primary school in Gloucestershire
What else?
Create an attachment-based nurture group with these detailed resources.
bit.ly/NurtureGroup
Use this mind map to identify the many issues faced by students with attachment disorders.
bit.ly/AttachmentDisorder
References
Bergin, C and Bergin, D (2009) “Attachment in the classroom”, Educational Psychology Review, 21: 141-70.
O’Connor, M and Russell, A (2004) “Identifying the incidence of psychological trauma and post-trauma
symptoms in children, A Survey of Three Clackmannanshire Schools”, Clackmannanshire Council
Psychological Service.
Subject: Children & youth; Students; Theory; Learning;
Publication title: The Times Educational Supplement
Issue: 5120
Publication year: 2014
Publication date: Nov 7, 2014
Year: 2014
Section: News
Publisher: TES Global Limited
Place of publication: London
Country of publication: United Kingdom
Publication subject: Education
ISSN: 00407887
Source type: Trade Journals
Language of publication: English
Document type: News
ProQuest document ID: 1625312946
Document URL: http://nclive.org/cgi-
bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1625312946?accountid=11330
Copyright: Copyright TSL Education Ltd. Nov 7, 2014
Last updated: 2014-11-17
Database: ProQuest Central
_______________________________________________________________
07 April 2016 Page 3 of 4 ProQuest
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1625312946?accountid=11330
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/1625312946?accountid=11330
Contact ProQuest
Copyright 2016 ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. – Terms and Conditions
07 April 2016 Page 4 of 4 ProQuest
http://www.proquest.com/go/pqissupportcontact
http://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditions
Clearinghouse on Elementary and
Early Childhood Education
University of Illinois • 51 Gerty Drive • Champaign, IL 61820-7469 ERIC DIGEST
(217) 333-1386 • (800) 583-4135 (voice/TTY) • ericeece@uiuc.edu • http://ericeece.org December 2003 • EDO-PS-03-17
for Infants and Toddlers
Debby Cryer, Sarah Hurwitz, and Mark Wolery
According to the 1999 National Survey of American
Families, an estimated 10% of infants under a year of age
are participating in center-based care and education
programs, while the numbers are higher for 1-year-olds
(16%) and 2-year-olds (24%) (Ehrle, Adams, & Tout,
2001). The practices used in caring for these infants and
toddlers may have strong and enduring impacts on
children’s development and well-being. In terms of child
care quality during the first three years, higher quality is
associated with better mother-child relationships, fewer
reports of children’s behavior problems, higher cognitive
and language outcomes, and better readiness for school
(Burchinal et al., 1996; NICHD Early Child Care Research
Network, 1996). In addition to the general quality of care
for infants and toddlers, discrete practices may influence
the development of infants and toddlers. One practice that
is currently attracting substantial interest is the provision of
continuity of caregivers for young children. Continuity of
caregivers means that infants and toddlers remain with
the same teacher(s) during a significant part, if not all, of
their first years in a program.
Continuity of Caregiver
Traditionally, young children in center-based child care
programs have a series of different caregivers during the
first three years of life. Centers often follow the lock-step
elementary school practice of moving children to a
different class/teacher at the end of the year. Many
programs move children more often, from class to class,
teacher to teacher, as soon as they attain certain
developmental milestones, such as crawling or walking.
Some programs may move children on a daily basis to
meet ratio or other staffing requirements. This practice is
often used to ensure efficient use of program resources
by keeping classes full and enrolling infants, for whom
there is more child care demand. High rates of teacher
turnover increase the likelihood that children will change
teachers repeatedly during the infant/toddler years
(Helburn, 1995; Whitebook, Howes, & Phillips, 1989).
The rationale for continuity of caregiver is similar to that for
assigning primary caregivers to very young children.
Primary caregivers take major responsibility for meeting
the care and educational needs of a small group of children,
within a larger group. Both practices, continuity of caregiver
and primary caregiver, are intended to create a consistent
personal relationship between a child and a teacher.
In these practices (primary caregiver and continuity of
caregiver), transitions between teachers are minimized
because transitions are seen as being stressful for the
child (and adults) and wasteful in terms of learning time.
When a child is moved to a new caregiver, recommended
practice suggests that strategies be used to ease transi-
tions. For example, children can visit their new class and
teacher before moving, or their new teacher can visit them
a few times so that they can get to know one another.
Theory and Research
The current professional recommendation of continuity of
caregiver for infants and toddlers is based on conclusions
drawn from child development theory and from limited
research findings. Theoretically, issues regarding the
development of secure maternal attachment are
considered paramount for infants and toddlers (Ainsworth
et al., 1978; Bowlby, 1982; Smith & Pederson, 1988).
Secure maternal relationships are associated with more
positive child outcomes, especially with regard to social-
emotional development (e.g., Matas, Arend, & Sroufe,
1978; Jacobson & Wille, 1986). There also is evidence
that maternal attachment is related to children’s language
development (Klann-Delius & Hofmeister, 1997; van
Ijzendoorn et al., 1995), cognitive development (van
Ijzendoorn et al., 1995), and emergent literacy (Bus & van
Ijzendoorn, 1988).
Some evidence suggests that in addition to attachment to
mothers, the child’s attachment to a primary caregiver in
out-of-home child care is also important (Cummings,
1980; Goossens & van Ijzendoorn, 1990; Howes &
Hamilton, 1992). Raikes’ (1993) research suggests that
children take a significant amount of time to form
attachments to caregivers, so they are less likely to form
attachments if frequent caregiver changes occur. Howes
and Hamilton (1992) found that with multiple changes in
caregivers, toddlers are less likely to relate to a new
caregiver based on her own behavior but rather re-create
the quality of the relationship with a previous caregiver.
They also report a relationship between the number of
caregiver losses experienced by a preschooler and the
likelihood that the child will be socially withdrawn or
aggressive with peers (Howes & Hamilton, 1993). It is
possible that the effects of caregiver changes might relate
to other areas of children’s development as well.
Current Practice
The extent to which children change caregivers during the
first three years in child care centers is not known, but it is
assumed to be high (Howes & Hamilton, 1993). Likewise,
little is known about the extent to which continuity of
caregiver is practiced, although reports of survey research
conducted by Cryer et al. (2000) of 273 centers indicate
that relatively few programs, whether accredited or non-
accredited, provide continuity of caregivers for infants and
even fewer provide it for toddlers.
Implementing Continuity of Caregiver
When the practice of continuity of caregiver is
implemented in child care centers, various strategies are
used. For example, the amount of time that children
remain with the same teacher might vary, with some
having the same teacher through the first and second
years, and others having the same teacher for a shorter
but extended period (e.g., 18 months). Keeping children
with the same teacher is more likely when multiage
groups are used, because having a birthday or reaching
developmental milestones does not force a change in
class. Continuity of caregiver, however, is also used with
same-age groupings. Teachers and their children may
use the same physical space through their years together,
or they may move from one classroom to another. In
classes with multiple teachers, all teachers and children
might move together, while in another setting, a subgroup
of children might move with only one of the teachers.
Thus, even within this practice (continuity of caregivers),
there can be substantial variation. Yet the major
requirement for providing continuity of caregiver is met.
To offer continuity of care for infants and toddlers, center
staff might want to consider the following suggestions:
• Avoid taking new children only in the youngest group;
this practice forces moving children up one at a time
and separates them from the teacher to whom they
are attached.
• Recruit new children to fill in at upper age levels when
it is more appropriate to have more children per adult.
• Use mixed-age groupings.
• Reward staff for longevity with the program.
• If a staff member leaves, overlap staff so that children
are never left with strangers.
Conclusion
Although positive child development effects may be
associated with the practice of continuity of caregiver, it is
certainly possible that there are also negative effects
associated with the practice. For example, if a child
spends several years with a teacher who interacts
negatively with the child, undesirable outcomes would be
likely. At this time, the actual effects associated with the
practice are based only on theoretical assumption and
limited research. Center staff may require more
compelling evidence that a practice is truly a better option
before undertaking the substantial modifications that are
required in making a significant change.
For More Information
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange
Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment.
New York: Basic.
Burchinal, M., Roberts, J., Nabors, L., & Bryant, D. (1996).
Quality of center child care and infant cognitive and
language development. Child Development, 67(2), 606-620.
EJ 523 402.
Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1988). Mother-child
interactions, attachment and emergent literacy: A cross-
sectional study. Child Development, 59(5), 1262-1272. EJ
380 597.
Cryer, D., Hurwitz, S., & Wolery, M. (2000). Continuity of
caregiver for infants and toddlers in center-based child care:
Report on a survey of center practices. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 15(4), 497-514. EJ 635 662.
Cummings, E. M. (1980). Caregiver stability and day care.
Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 31-37.
Ehrle, J., Adams, G., & Tout, K. (2001). Who’s caring for our
youngest children? Child care patterns of infants and
toddlers. Washington, DC: Urban Institute. ED 448 908.
Essa, E. L., Favre, K., Thweatt, G., & Waugh, S. (1999).
Continuity of care for infants and toddlers. Early
Development and Care, 148, 11-19.
Goossens, F. A., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1990). Quality of
infants’ attachments to professional caregivers: Relation to
infant-parent and day-care characteristics. Child Develop-
ment, 61(3), 832-837. EJ 413 810.
Helburn, S. W. (Ed.). (1995). Cost, quality, and child
outcomes in child care centers. Technical report. Denver:
University of Colorado at Denver. ED 386 297.
Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1992). Children’s relationships
with caregivers: Mothers and child care teachers. Child
Development, 63(4), 859-866. EJ 450 556.
Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1993). The changing
experience of child care: Changes in teachers and in
teacher-child relationships and children’s social competence
with peers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8(1), 15-32.
EJ 461 737.
Jacobson, J. L., & Wille, D. E. (1986). The influence of
attachment pattern on developmental changes in peer
interaction from the toddler to the preschool period. Child
Development, 57(2), 338-347. EJ 357 931.
Klann-Delius, G., & Hofmeister, C. (1997). The development
of communicative competence of securely and insecurely
attached children in interactions with their mothers. Journal
of Psycholinguistic Research, 26(1), 69-88.
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. (1978). Continuity of
adaptation in the second year: The relationship between
quality of attachment and later competence. Child Develop-
ment, 49(3), 547-556. EJ 190 989.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1996).
Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to
positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
11(3), 269-306. EJ 534 662.
Raikes, H. (1993). Relationship duration in infant care: Time
with a high-ability teacher and infant-teacher attachment. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 8(3), 309-325. EJ 474 787.
Smith, P. B., & Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity
and patterns of infant-mother attachment. Child Develop-
ment, 59(4), 1097-1101. EJ 377 898.
van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Dijkstra, J., & Bus, A. G. (1995).
Attachment, intelligence and language: A meta-analysis.
Social Development, 4(2), 115-128.
Whitebook, M., Howes, C., & Phillips, D. (1989). Who cares?
Child care teachers and the quality of care in America. Final
report of the National Child Care Staffing Study. Oakland,
CA: Child Care Employee Project.
____________________
References identified with an ED (ERIC document), EJ (ERIC journal), or PS
number are cited in the ERIC database. Most documents are available in ERIC
microfiche collections at more than 1,000 locations worldwide (see
http://www.ed.gov/Programs/EROD/). They can also be ordered through EDRS:
800-443-ERIC or online at http://www.edrs.com/Express.cfm. Journal articles are
available from the original journal, interlibrary loan services, or article reproduction
clearinghouses such as Ingenta (800-296-2221).
ERIC Digests are in the public domain and may be freely reproduced.
This project has been funded at least in part with Federal funds from the U.S.
Department of Education, under contract number ED-99-CO-0020. The content
of this publication does not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the U.S.
Department of Education, nor does mention of trade names, commercial
products, or organizations imply endorsement by the U.S. Government.
Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition at Ages 3 and 7 Years
Effects of Breastfeeding Duration and Exclusivity
Mandy B. Belfort, MD, MPH; Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman, MPH; Ken P. Kleinman, ScD; Lauren B. Guthrie, MPH;
David C. Bellinger, PhD; Elsie M. Taveras, MD, MPH; Matthew W. Gillman, MD, SM; Emily Oken, MD, MPH
IMPORTANCE Breastfeeding may benefit child cognitive development, but few studies have
quantified breastfeeding duration or exclusivity, nor has any study to date examined the role
of maternal diet during lactation on child cognition.
OBJECTIVES To examine relationships of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity with child
cognition at ages 3 and 7 years and to evaluate the extent to which maternal fish intake
during lactation modifies associations of infant feeding with later cognition.
DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Prospective cohort study (Project Viva), a US prebirth
cohort that enrolled mothers from April 22, 1999, to July 31, 2002, and followed up children
to age 7 years, including 1312 Project Viva mothers and children.
MAIN EXPOSURE Duration of any breastfeeding to age 12 months.
MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Child receptive language assessed with the Peabody
Picture Vocabulary Test at age 3 years, Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities at
ages 3 and 7 years, and Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test and Wide Range Assessment of
Memory and Learning at age 7 years.
RESULTS Adjusting for sociodemographics, maternal intelligence, and home environment in
linear regression, longer breastfeeding duration was associated with higher Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test score at age 3 years (0.21; 95% CI, 0.03-0.38 points per month breastfed)
and with higher intelligence on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test at age 7 years (0.35;
0.16-0.53 verbal points per month breastfed; and 0.29; 0.05-0.54 nonverbal points per
month breastfed). Breastfeeding duration was not associated with Wide Range Assessment
of Memory and Learning scores. Beneficial effects of breastfeeding on the Wide Range
Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities at age 3 years seemed greater for women who
consumed 2 or more servings of fish per week (0.24; 0.00-0.47 points per month breastfed)
compared with less than 2 servings of fish per week (−0.01; −0.22 to 0.20 points per month
breastfed) (P = .16 for interaction).
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Our results support a causal relationship of breastfeeding
duration with receptive language and verbal and nonverbal intelligence later in life.
JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(9):836-844. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.45
5
Published online July 29, 2013.
Editorial page 79
6
Author Affiliations: Division of
Newborn Medicine, Boston Children’s
Hospital, Harvard Medical School,
Boston, Massachusetts (Belfort);
Department of Population Medicine,
Harvard Medical School and Harvard
Pilgrim Health Care Institute, Boston,
Massachusetts (Rifas-Shiman,
Kleinman, Guthrie, Taveras, Gillman,
Oken); Department of Neurology,
Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard
Medical School, Boston,
Massachusetts (Bellinger);
Departments of Epidemiology and
Nutrition, Harvard School of Public
Health, Boston, Massachusetts
(Gillman).
Corresponding Author: Mandy B.
Belfort, MD, MPH, Division of
Newborn Medicine, Boston Children’s
Hospital, Harvard Medical School,
Hunnewell Room 438, 30
0
Longwood Ave, Boston, MA 02115
(mandy.belfort@childrens.harvard
.edu).
Research
Original Investigation
836 jamapediatrics.com
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
S trong evidence supports the relationship between breast-
feeding and health benefits in infancy, including pre-
vention of gastrointestinal tract infections and otitis
media.1 The extent to which breastfeeding leads to better cog-
nitive development is less certain. While observational
studies1-4 have reported positive associations of breastfeed-
ing with later intelligence, breastfeeding is strongly related to
determinants of child intelligence, such as maternal intelli-
gence, and developmental stimulation received by the child;
residual confounding by such shared determinants may have
led observational studies1,2,5 to overestimate the effect of
breastfeeding on child intelligence. Another limitation of prior
investigations is the classification of infant feeding as ever
vs never breastfed.4 Failure to account for partial vs exclu-
sive breastfeeding or breastfeeding duration could lead to
underestimation of the true effect of breastfeeding on child
intelligence. Detailed data regarding breastfeeding exposure
and adequate control for confounding factors are necessary
for valid estimates of the relationship between breastfeed-
ing and later intelligence, but no study to date has fulfilled
these requirements.
Nutrients in breast milk, such as n-3 fatty acid docosa-
hexaenoic acid (DHA), may benefit the developing brain.
A major determinant of breast milk DHA content is
the mother’s diet,6 and fish is a rich source of DHA. In
pregnancy, greater maternal fish intake (particularly fish
low in mercury contamination) is associated with better
childhood cognitive outcomes,7 but the extent to which
maternal fish intake during lactation accounts for the
relationship between breastfeeding and cognition has not
been reported. The aims of our study were 2-fold: (1) to
examine relationships of breastfeeding duration and ex-
clusivity with child cognition at ages 3 and 7 years and (2) to
evaluate the extent to which maternal fish intake during
lactation modifies associations of infant feeding with later
cognition.
Methods
Participants
We studied participants in Project Viva, a prospective, longi-
tudinal cohort study designed to examine prenatal factors
in relation to pregnancy and child health. From April 22,
1999, to July 31, 2002, Project Viva enrolled pregnant
women attending prenatal care at 8 obstetrical offices of a
multispecialty group practice in eastern Massachusetts.
Exclusion criteria included multiple gestation, inability to
answer questions in English, gestational age of at least 2
2
weeks at the initial prenatal care appointment, and plans to
move away from the area before delivery. Recruitment and
follow-up details at birth,8 at 6 months,9 and at 3 years10
have been reported. Follow-up data collection at age 7 years
was completed in December 2010. Human investigation
committees of the Harvard Pilgrim Health Care Institute,
Brigham and Women’s Hospital, and Beth Israel Deaconess
Medical Center approved the study, and mothers of all par-
ticipating children gave written informed consent.
Of 2128 women who delivered a live infant, we excluded
45 children born at a gestational age of less than 34 weeks, 325
children who were missing breastfeeding status at age 6 months
and breastfeeding duration at age 12 months, and 446 chil-
dren who were missing cognitive measures at ages 3 and 7 years.
Therefore, our sample for this analysis comprised 1312 Proj-
ect Viva mothers and children (1224 at age 3 years and 1037 at
age 7 years).
Measurements
Breastfeeding
When the participating child was ages 6 and 12 months, we
asked the mother the questions listed in Table 1. To deter-
mine breastfeeding exclusivity at ages 6 and 12 months, we
asked detailed questions about the age at which solid foods
and non–breast milk liquids were introduced.
Cognition
When children were age 3 years, trained research staff
administered the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third
Edition (PPVT-III),11 a test of receptive language correlated
(Pearson R = 0.90) with intelligence tests, such as the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III. We also admin-
istered the Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities
(WRAVMA)12 pegboard (fine motor), matching (visual spa-
tial), and drawing (visual motor) subtests. Subtest scores are
reported individually and combined as a visual motor com-
posite score.
At age 7 years, we administered the WRAVMA drawing sub-
test and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition
(KBIT-II), which measures verbal and nonverbal intelligen
ce
and is correlated (Pearson R = 0.89) with the Wechsler Intel-
ligence Scale for Children III.13 In addition, we assessed memory
and learning with the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and
Learning (WRAML)14 design memory and picture memory
tests. Scores were summed to yield a visual memory com-
bined score.
Table 1. Questions About Breastfeeding at Ages 6 and 12 Months
Question About Breastfeeding
At Age 6 mo
For all infants
(1) Have you ever breastfed your baby? By breastfeeding, we mean that
you have put your baby to your breast, whether or not your baby actually
received breast milk, or that you have fed your baby your breast milk.
(2) Are you now feeding your baby any infant formula?
(3) Are you now feeding your baby any breast milk?
For weaned infants
How old was your baby when you stopped breastfeeding?
At Age 12 mo
For all infants
Have you ever breastfed your child?
Are you still breastfeeding at all?
For weaned infants
How old was your child when you stopped breastfeeding?
Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition Original Investigation Research
jamapediatrics.com JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 83
7
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
Study staff administering cognitive tests were unaware of
the children’s breastfeeding status. The PPVT-III, WRAVMA,
and KBIT-II are scaled to a mean (SD) score of 100 (15).
Covariates
We collected data from mothers regarding parental and child
demographic, social, economic, and health information
through self-administered questionnaires and interviews in
pregnancy and shortly after delivery.15 At 6 months’ post par-
tum, we administered a brief, validated food frequency
questionnaire,16 including questions about the mother’s mean
weekly fish intake (canned tuna fish, shellfish, and dark meat
fish [eg, mackerel, salmon, sardines, bluefish, or swordfish],
as well as other fish [eg, cod, haddock, or halibut]) since the
infant’s birth. To measure maternal intelligence, we adminis-
tered to mothers the PPVT-III when the child was age 3 years
and the KBIT-II when the child was age 7 years. We also ad-
ministered the Home Observation Measurement of the Envi-
ronment short form (HOME-SF),17 which measures cognitive
stimulation and emotional support in the child’s environ-
ment. Higher scores (range, 0-22) indicate more favorable en-
vironments.
Data Analysis
Our main exposures were the following: (1) duration of any
breastfeeding in months; (2) duration of exclusive breastfeed-
ing in months, defined as feeding breast milk but no solid foods
or non–breast milk liquids (except water) to age 6 months; and
(3) breastfeeding status at age 6 months, categorized as “for-
mula only, never breast fed,” “formula only, weaned,” “mixed
formula and breast milk,” and “breast milk only, no formula.”
Our outcome measures were the PPVT-III and WRAVMA scores
at age 3 years and the KBIT-II, WRAVMA, and WRAML scores
at age 7 years. To examine the effect of potential confounders
on estimated relationships of breastfeeding measures with cog-
nitive outcome measures, we adjusted for 4 models in linear
regression. Model 0 adjusted for child age and sex. Model 1 ad-
justed for covariates in model 0 plus gestational age and birth
weight z score.18 Model 2 adjusted for covariates in model 1 plus
child race/ethnicity and maternal age, parity, smoking status,
depression at 6 months’ post partum, and employment and
child care at age 6 months, as well as primary language, an-
nual household income, and parental educational level and
marital status. Model 3 adjusted for covariates in model 2 plus
HOME-SF score. Model 4 adjusted for covariates in model 3 plus
maternal PPVT-III or KBIT-II score.
To compare our results with those of other studies, we es-
timated the difference in cognitive test scores between chil-
dren ever vs never breastfed. To examine the extent to which
maternal fish intake modified relationships of breastfeeding
with outcome measures, we stratified by fish intake (<2 vs ≥2
servings per week) and calculated the P value for an interac-
tion term (breastfeeding duration × fish intake) in linear re-
gression.
All covariates were not observed in all participants. Using
only individuals with all data observed would have resulted
in a smaller sample size, with most excluded participants miss-
ing only 1 or 2 values, leading to lost information and possibly
a selected subset. Therefore, we used multiple imputation to
generate several plausible values for each missing value.19 To
generate imputation data sets, we used a set of variables cho-
sen from the thousands available in Project Viva to reflect
demographic and other factors that we deemed plausibly re-
lated to potential missingness mechanisms and to the expo-
sures and outcome measures. A “completed” data set in-
cludes the observed data and an imputed value for each missing
value. The analysis was replicated across completed data sets
and then combined in a structured fashion that accurately re-
flects the true amount of information in the observed data.
This method assumes that the exposures and outcome mea-
sures are missing completely at random given the observed
variables and the imputed covariates. This is a reduced as-
sumption relative to that made in studies that use only com-
plete cases. Using a statistical program (Proc MI ANALYZE in
SAS, version 9.3; SAS Institute, Inc), we generated 50 complete
data sets and combined multivariable modeling results for all
2128 participants in the Project Viva cohort. For this analysis,
we excluded participants born at a gestational age of less than
34 weeks and those missing observed exposure or outcome
data.
Results
Table 2 summarizes characteristics of participants included at
ages 3 and 7 years and characteristics of the excluded partici-
pants. Compared with those included in the analysis, moth-
ers of excluded participants were less educated, had lower an-
nual household income, were more likely to be of nonwhite
race/ethnicity, and breastfed for a shorter duration. For 122
4
participants included at age 3 years, the mean duration of any
breastfeeding was 6.4 months and of exclusive breastfeeding
was 2.4 months; values were similar for participants in-
cluded at age 7 years. The PPVT-III mean score at age 3 years
was 103.7, and the KBIT-II verbal mean score at age 7 years was
112.5.
Table 3 summarizes the effect of covariate adjustment on
estimated relationships between breastfeeding duration and
child cognitive outcomes. Adjusting for child age and sex
(model 0), longer breastfeeding duration was associated with
higher PPVT-III score at age 3 years (0.58; 95% CI, 0.40-0.76
points per month breastfed). This relationship was similar with
additional adjustment for fetal growth and gestational age
(model 1) and attenuated with adjustment for demographic
variables (model 2) and HOME-SF score (model 3). With fur-
ther adjustment for maternal IQ (model 4), the association di-
minished to 0.21 (95% CI, 0.03-0.38) points per month breast-
fed. We observed a similar pattern of attenuation for the KBIT-II
verbal and nonverbal scores at age 7 years.
In Table 4, we give fully adjusted associations of any and
exclusive breastfeeding with all cognitive test scores at ages
3
and 7 years. Associations of breastfeeding duration (any and
exclusive) with the PPVT-III score and the KBIT-II verbal and
nonverbal scores were positive, and 95% CIs excluded 0. The
Figure shows the adjusted KBIT-II scores at age 7 years by cat-
egory of any breastfeeding duration (<1, 1-3, 4-6, 7-9, 10-11, and
Research Original Investigation Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition
838 JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 jamapediatrics.com
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
Table 2. Included and Excluded Project Viva Mothers and Children
Variable
Included at Age 3
y
(n = 1224)
Included at Age 7 y
(n = 1037)
Excluded at Ages 3 and 7 ya
(n = 771)
Mother
Age, mean (SE), y 32.5 (0.1) 32.3 (0.2) 31.0 (0.2)
PPVT-III score, mean (SE) 106.0 (0.4) 105.4 (0.5) 100.8 (0.8)
KBIT-II score, mean (SE) 107.9 (0.5) 107.2 (0.5) 102.5 (0.7)
≥2 Servings of fish per wk, % 55.0 52.8 50.
1
Parity, %
0 47.5 47.3 47.6
1 36.3 36.4 35.4
≥2 16.2 16.4 17.0
Smoking status, %
Never 68.8 70.1 64.5
Former 21.0 20.1 17.4
During pregnancy 10.2 9.8 18.1
Depression at 6 mo post partum, % 8.8 9.1 11.0
Educational level, %
≤High school diploma 7.2 8.6 18.8
Some college 20.2 21.3 26.9
Bachelor’s degree 37.7 34.9 32.7
Graduate degree 34.9 35.2 21.6
Employment at 6 mo post partum, %
Employed 66.0 66.7 56.0
Employed, on maternity leave 6.0 6.8 6.2
Not employed, looking 4.6 5.3 10.1
Not employed, not looking 23.3 21.3 27.7
Child
Gestational age, mean (SE), wk 39.6 (0.0) 39.7 (0.0) 39.6 (0.1)
Birth weight, mean (SE), kg 3.5 (0.0) 3.5 (0.0) 3.5 (0.0)
Birth weight for gestational age z
score, mean (SE)
0.22 (0.03) 0.20 (0.00) 0.14 (0.04)
Female sex, % 50.4 50.5 47.7
Race/ethnicity, %
Asian 2.7 3.0 5.8
Black 11.9 15.1 21.0
Hispanic 3.5 3.7 8.0
White 70.1 66.1 55.4
Other 11.8 12.2 9.8
Primary English speaker, % 96.2 98.7 92.7
Child care at age 6 mo, %
Center 16.8 17.2 17.4
Other home 26.1 27.3 24.8
At own home 14.3 13.6 14.9
None 42.8 42.0 43.0
Breastfeeding status at age 6 mo, %
Formula only, never breastfed 10.6 10.1 15.0
Formula only, weaned 34.7 35.1 46.3
Mixed formula and breast milk 26.4 26.8 19.5
Breast milk only, no formula 28.2 28.0 19.2
(continued)
Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition Original Investigation Research
jamapediatrics.com JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 839
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
≥12 months). Associations of breastfeeding duration with
WRAVMA scores were null (with narrow 95% CIs).
Estimated cognitive test mean score differences accord-
ing to breastfeeding status at age 6 months are given in Table 5.
Compared with children fed breast milk only, the PPVT-III score
at age 3 years was approximately 3 points lower for children
never breastfed and approximately 2 points lower for weaned
children and those receiving mixed feedings (P = .01 for trend).
We found a similar trend for the KBIT-II verbal and nonverbal
scores at age 7 years but observed no appreciable trend for the
WRAVMA or WRAML scores.
Compared with children who were never breastfed, the
fully adjusted PPVT-III score at age 3 years was 1.45 (95% CI,
−0.98 to 3.87) points higher for children who were ever breast-
Table 3. Effect of Covariate Adjustment on Estimated Associations of Duration of Any Breastfeeding With Child Cognitiona
Model
Points (95% CI) per Month Breastfed
At Age 3 y
(n = 1224)
At Age 7 y
(n = 1037)
PPVT-III Score KBIT-II Verbal Score KBIT-II Nonverbal
Score
0, Child age and sex 0.58 (0.40 to 0.76) 0.96 (0.77 to 1.14) 0.65 (0.43 to 0.87)
1, Model 0 plus fetal growth and gestational age 0.57 (0.39 to 0.75) 0.95 (0.76 to 1.14) 0.64 (0.42 to 0.86)
2, Model 1 plus demographic variablesb 0.29 (0.12 to 0.47) 0.46 (0.28 to 0.65) 0.38 (0.13 to 0.62)
3, Model 2 plus HOME-SF score 0.29 (0.11 to 0.46) 0.46 (0.27 to 0.64) 0.38 (0.13 to 0.62)
4, Model 3 plus maternal IQ 0.21 (0.03 to 0.38) 0.35 (0.16 to 0.53) 0.29 (0.05 to 0.54)
Abbreviations: HOME-SF, Home Observation Measurement of the Environment
short form; KBIT-II, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition; PPVT-III,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition.
a Any breastfeeding through age 12 months.
b Child race/ethnicity and maternal age, parity, smoking status, depression,
employment, and child care at 6 months’ post partum, as well as parental
educational level and marital status, annual household income, and primary
language.
Table 2. Included and Excluded Project Viva Mothers and Children (continued)
Variable
Included at Age 3
y
(n = 1224)
Included at Age 7 y
(n = 1037)
Excluded at Ages 3 and 7 ya
(n = 771)
Duration of breastfeeding, mean
(SE), mo
Exclusive to age 6 mo 2.4 (0.1) 2.4 (0.1) 1.9 (0.1)
Any to age 12 mo 6.4 (0.1) 6.5 (0.1) 4.9 (0.2)
Cognitive test score, mean (SE)
At age 3 y
PPVT-III 103.7 (0.43) NA NA
WRAVMA total 101.8 (0.33) NA NA
At age 7 y
KBIT-II verbal NA 112.5 (0.5) NA
KBIT-II nonverbal NA 106.5 (0.5) NA
WRAVMA drawing NA 92.2 (0.5) NA
WRAML visual memory NA 16.9 (0.1) NA
Family or Household
HOME-SF score, mean (SE) 18.4 (0.1) 18.4 (0.1) NA
Annual household income during
first trimester, %
<$40 000 14.3 16.2 23.2
$40 001-$70 000 22.3 21.2 26.4
>$70 000 63.4 62.6 50.4
Married or cohabitating during first
trimester, %
93.6 92.4 89.3
Mother’s Partner
Educational level, %
≤High school diploma 13.3 14.2 22.1
Some college 19.8 20.5 24.7
Bachelor’s degree 36.5 35.7 30.0
Graduate degree 30.4 29.6 23.3
Abbreviations: HOME-SF, Home
Observation Measurement of the
Environment short form; KBIT-II,
Kaufman Brief Intelligence
Test–Second Edition; NA, not
applicable; PPVT-III, Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test–Third Edition;
WRAML, Wide Range Assessment of
Memory and Learning; WRAVMA,
Wide Range Assessment of Visual
Motor Abilities.
a Excluded because of missing
exposure or outcome data.
Research Original Investigation Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition
840 JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 jamapediatrics.com
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
fed, and the KBIT-II verbal score at age 7 years was 3.75 (1.17-
6.33) points higher. The WRAVMA and WRAML scores were not
statistically different (data not shown).
Stratifying by maternal postpartum fish intake (<2 vs ≥2 servings per week), the relationship between breastfeeding du- ration and the WRAVMA score at age 3 years seemed stronger in children of women with higher vs lower fish intake (Table 6), but the interaction was not statistically significant (P = .16 for interaction). For other cognitive outcomes, associations with breastfeeding duration were not appreciably stronger among children of women who consumed more fish.
Discussion
We found that longer duration of breastfeeding and greater
exclusivity of breastfeeding were associated with better
receptive language at age 3 years and with higher verbal and
nonverbal IQ at age 7 years. At age 7 years, the effect size of
0.35 verbal IQ points per month of any breastfeeding trans-
lates to 4.2 points, or almost one-third of an SD during 12
months, whereas the effect size of 0.80 verbal IQ points per
month of exclusive breastfeeding translates to almost 5
points over 6 months. Effects were similar in direction but
somewhat weaker in magnitude for nonverbal IQ and recep-
tive language at age 3 years. We found no important main
association of breastfeeding with visual motor skills or
visual memory.
While numerous investigations have demonstrated asso-
ciations of breastfeeding with later cognition, some studies1,4,5
have had methodological flaws. In particular, adequate con-
trol for confounding factors is critical because breastfeeding
and child cognition share many determinants, including ma-
ternal characteristics and environmental factors. A 2007
meta-analysis1 identified maternal intelligence and the home
environment as key confounders that are frequently over-
looked and found only 1 prior study5 with appropriate adjust-
ment, an analysis of data from the US National Longitudinal
Survey of Youth (NSLY) in which the association of breastfeed-
ing (ever vs never) with achievement scores at ages 5 to 14 years
was attenuated from 4.7 to 1.3 points after adjustment for ma-
ternal intelligence and diminished to only 0.5 points after ad-
justment for sociodemographic and other variables, includ-
ing the HOME-SF score. We also adjusted for maternal
intelligence and the HOME-SF score, as well as numerous other
potential confounders, and nevertheless found a substan-
tially stronger association (3.75 points) of ever vs never breast-
fed with verbal IQ at age 7 years.
It is possible that differences in the degree of breastfeed-
ing exclusivity explain why we observed a stronger associa-
tion of breastfeeding with cognition than was seen in the
NSLY.5 By classifying breastfeeding as ever vs never, the NSLY
may have included in their breastfed group a substantial
number of infants who received formula and breast milk,
biasing results toward the null, but the authors did not report
the degree of mixed feedings. Differences in breastfeeding
duration may also explain our discrepant results. In a second-
ary analysis, the NSLY found that the achievement scores of
children breastfed for at least 29 weeks were 1.5 points higher
than those of children never breastfed (P = .01), but the
authors considered their data about breastfeeding duration
“less reliable” than data about whether a child was ever
Figure. Differences in Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition
Verbal Scores at Age 7 Years According to Duration of Any
Breastfeeding, With Linear Trend Line
0
1-3 4-6 7-9 10-11 ≥12
7
5
6
Ve
rb
al
IQ
D
iff
er
en
ce
Duration of Any Breastfeeding, mo
4
2
3
1
<1
Estimates are adjusted for child age, sex, fetal growth, gestational age,
race/ethnicity, and primary language and for maternal age, parity, smoking
status, IQ, depression, employment, and child care at 6 months’ post partum, as
well as for parental education level, annual household income, and Home
Observation Measurement of the Environment short form score.
Table 4. Adjusted Associations of Duration of Breastfeeding With
Cognitive Test Scoresa
Score
Points (95% CI) per Month Breastfed
Any Breastfeeding
to Age 12 mo
Exclusive Breastfeeding
to Age 6 mob
At Age 3 y
PPVT-III 0.21 (0.03 to 0.38) 0.50 (0.11 to 0.89)
WRAVMA drawing 0.01 (−0.15 to 0.16) −0.12 (−0.47 to 0.22)
WRAVMA pegboard 0.09 (−0.06 to 0.24) −0.03 (−0.37 to 0.31)
WRAVMA matching 0.09 (−0.10 to 0.27) 0.00 (−0.42 to 0.41)
WRAVMA total 0.08 (−0.07 to 0.23) −0.07 (−0.40 to 0.27)
At Age 7 y
KBIT-II verbal 0.35 (0.16 to 0.53) 0.80 (0.38 to 1.22)
KBIT-II nonverbal 0.29 (0.05 to 0.54) 0.58 (0.01 to 1.14)
WRAVMA drawing −0.08 (−0.33 to 0.18) −0.05 (−0.62 to 0.53)
WRAML visual
memory
0.04 (−0.02 to 0.11) 0.12 (−0.03 to 0.27)
Abbreviations: HOME-SF, Home Observation Measurement of the Environment
short form; KBIT-II, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition; PPVT-III,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition; WRAML, Wide Range
Assessment of Memory and Learning; WRAVMA, Wide Range Assessment of
Visual Motor Abilities.
a Estimates are adjusted for child age, sex, fetal growth, gestational age,
race/ethnicity, and primary language and for maternal age, parity, smoking
status, IQ, depression, employment, and child care at 6 months’ post partum,
as well as for parental educational level, annual household income, and
HOME-SF score.
b No solid foods or non–breast milk liquids (except water).
Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition Original Investigation Research
jamapediatrics.com JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 841
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
breastfed. Finally, variable outcome measures (achievement
test score in the NSLY vs IQ in our study) may explain our dif-
ferent results.
We identified 4 additional observational studies20-23 that
adjusted for maternal intelligence and the HOME-SF score.
While we found a modest association of breastfeeding with
verbal intelligence at age 3 years, neither of the other 2 pre-
school studies found an important association with cogni-
tive outcomes (McCarthy General Cognitive Index21,22 and
PPVT-Revised22 at age 4 years). Of the studies reporting
school-age outcomes, one study21 found a 1.3-point (95% CI,
−2.3 to 4.9) advantage of ever vs never breastfeeding on the
Wechsler Full-Scale IQ at age 7 years; another study23 found
a 0.7-point (0.2-1.3) advantage on the same outcome at age 11
years; and the other study22 found no association with ver-
bal or performance IQ at age 11 years (effect estimate not
reported). All those effect estimates are smaller than ours,
but none of the studies accounted for breastfeeding duration
or exclusivity.
Studies of cohorts with different confounding patterns are
also informative. Brion et al24 analyzed associations of breast-
feeding duration with IQ at age 8 years in 2 cohorts. In one co-
hort (the British Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Chil-
dren), breastfeeding duration and child IQ were strongly
predicted by measures of socioeconomic position, whereas in
the other cohort (Pelotas, Brazil) child IQ was predicted by so-
cioeconomic factors, but breastfeeding duration was not. In
both cohorts, child IQ was strongly associated with breast-
feeding duration, suggesting that confounding alone did not
explain the relationship.
The results of our study are also consistent with a large clus-
ter randomized trial25 of breastfeeding promotion in which ver-
Table 5. Adjusted Cognitive Test Score Differences at Ages 3 and 7 Years According to Breastfeeding Status at Age 6 Monthsa
Score
Difference in Points (95% CI)
P Value for TrenddNever Breastfed Weaned Mixed Feedingsb Breast Milk Onlyc
At Age 3 y
PPVT-III −3.17 (−5.92 to −0.41) −2.26 (−4.22 to −0.29) −2.27 (−4.24 to −0.30) 0.00 [Reference] .01
WRAVMA drawing −0.63 (−3.03 to 1.78) −0.01 (−1.74 to 1.72) −0.52 (−2.27 to 1.22) 0.00 [Reference] .80
WRAVMA pegboard −1.96 (−4.31 to 0.39) −0.36 (−2.05 to 1.34) 0.04 (−1.66 to 1.74) 0.00 [Reference] .18
WRAVMA matching −1.93 (−4.82 to 0.96) −0.62 (−2.65 to 1.41) −1.13 (−3.21 to 0.96) 0.00 [Reference] .29
WRAVMA total −2.04 (−4.39 to 0.30) −0.47 (−2.13 to 1.19) −0.75 (−2.42 to 0.93) 0.00 [Reference] .19
At Age 7 y
KBIT-II verbal −5.59 (−8.52 to −2.67) −2.96 (−5.05 to −0.88) −1.40 (−3.49 to 0.68) 0.00 [Reference] <.001
KBIT-II nonverbal −2.71 (−6.62 to 1.21) −3.11 (−5.89 to −0.33) −1.33 (−4.08 to 1.43) 0.00 [Reference] .04
WRAVMA drawing 0.47 (−3.62 to 4.55) −0.14 (−3.03 to 2.76) −0.73 (−3.61 to 2.15) 0.00 [Reference] .87
WRAML visual memory −0.67 (−1.72 to 0.39) −0.30 (−1.06 to 0.45) 0.39 (−0.36 to 1.14) 0.00 [Reference] .15
Abbreviations: HOME-SF, Home Observation Measurement of the Environment
short form; KBIT-II, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition; PPVT-III,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition; WRAML, Wide Range
Assessment of Memory and Learning; WRAVMA, Wide Range Assessment of
Visual Motor Abilities.
a Estimates are adjusted for child age, sex, fetal growth, gestational age,
race/ethnicity, and primary language and for maternal age, parity, smoking
status, IQ, depression, employment, and child care at 6 months’ post partum,
as well as for parental educational level, annual household income, and
HOME-SF score.
b Breast milk and formula.
c No formula.
d P values are calculated in linear regression with breastfeeding category as a
continuous exposure variable (1 indicates never breastfed; 2, weaned; 3, mixed
feedings; and 4, breast milk only).
Table 6. Adjusted Associations of Duration of Breastfeeding With Cognitive Test Scores at Ages 3 and 7 Years by Postpartum Maternal Fish Intakea
Score
Points (95% CI) per Month Breastfed P Value for
Interaction<2 Servings of Fish per Week ≥2 Servings of Fish per Week
At Age 3 y
PPVT-III 0.10 (−0.15 to 0.35) 0.30 (0.03 to 0.58) .22
WRAVMA total −0.01 (−0.22 to 0.20) 0.24 (0.00 to 0.47) .16
At Age 7 y
KBIT-II verbal 0.31 (0.06 to 0.56) 0.35 (0.05 to 0.65) .82
KBIT-II nonverbal 0.36 (0.02 to 0.69) 0.15 (−0.25 to 0.56) .95
WRAVMA total −0.04 (−0.40 to 0.32) −0.06 (−0.47 to 0.35) .92
WRAML 0.06 (−0.03 to 0.16) 0.04 (−0.06 to 0.14) .75
Abbreviations: HOME-SF, Home Observation Measurement of the Environment
short form; KBIT-II, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test–Second Edition; PPVT-III,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition; WRAML, Wide Range
Assessment of Memory and Learning; WRAVMA, Wide Range Assessment of
Visual Motor Abilities.
a Estimates are adjusted for child age, sex, fetal growth, gestational age,
race/ethnicity, and primary language and for maternal age, parity, smoking
status, IQ, depression, employment, and child care at 6 months’ post partum,
as well as for parental educational level, annual household income, and
HOME-SF score.
Research Original Investigation Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition
842 JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 jamapediatrics.com
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
bal IQ at age 6.5 years was 7.5 points (one-half of an SD) higher
in the breastfeeding promotion group. By design, that study
minimized confounding by measured and unmeasured fac-
tors; however, nonblinding of clinicians assessing the cogni-
tive outcomes to participant breastfeeding status suggests the
potential for bias. Together, the results of the well-controlled
observational studies20-23 (including ours), the analysis of co-
horts without social patterning of breastfeeding (eg, in the Pelo-
tas cohort),24 and the large randomized trial25 suggest that con-
founding does not account fully for the observed association
of breastfeeding with later cognition.
In analyses stratified by fish intake, the beneficial effects
of breastfeeding on visual motor ability at age 3 years seemed
greater for women who consumed 2 or more servings com-
pared with less than 2 servings per week, although the inter-
action was not statistically significant. This observation is con-
sistent with the hypothesis that 1 or more nutrients in fish
transfer to breast milk and account for some of the observed
beneficial effect and is relevant to optimizing the maternal diet
during lactation. Docosahexaenoic acid is incorporated in large
amounts into cell membranes of the developing retina and
brain. Its content in breast milk is variable26 and depends on
DHA sources in the maternal diet,6,27 including fish; infant DHA
status in turn depends on the DHA content of ingested breast
milk.27 Randomized trials of DHA supplementation during lac-
tation have found beneficial effects of DHA on early motor
skills28 and sustained attention29 but not visual motor func-
tion or general cognition.28,30 Our observation may be ex-
plained by DHA or nutrients in fish other than DHA. It may also
be a chance finding.
Strengths of our study include a prospective design, de-
tailed contemporaneous measurement of duration and exclu-
sivity of breastfeeding, and measurement of numerous poten-
tial confounding variables, including the home environment and
maternal IQ. As in all observational studies, confounding by un-
measured factors is possible and may have led us to overesti-
mate the true effect of breastfeeding, although our results are
consistent with data from a randomized trial25 of breastfeed-
ing promotion that eliminates confounding by design. We mea-
sured cognition at school age, which tends to be stable through
adulthood31 compared with measurement in preschool or ear-
lier. The elevated socioeconomic status and high breastfeed-
ing rate of our cohort may limit generalizability of the study find-
ings. In addition, we followed up only a subset of the original
Project Viva cohort to ages 3 and 7 years. The children we ob-
served tended to be of higher socioeconomic status and were
less likely to be of minority race/ethnicity than the children we
did not follow up, which could have led to overestimates if the
effect of breastfeeding on cognition was much weaker or in the
opposite direction in those who dropped out, situations we find
unlikely. Finally, for the statistically significant associations of
breastfeeding with later cognition, 95% CIs were narrow and ex-
clude a null result, but the lower confidence limits include val-
ues with little clinical importance.
In summary, our results support a causal relationship of
breastfeeding in infancy with receptive language at age 3 and
with verbal and nonverbal IQ at school age. These findings sup-
port national and international recommendations to pro-
mote exclusive breastfeeding through age 6 months and con-
tinuation of breastfeeding through at least age 1 year.
ARTICLE INFORMATION
Accepted for Publication: February 6, 2013.
Published Online: July 29, 2013.
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.455.
Author Contributions: Study concept and design:
Belfort, Kleinman, Gillman, Oken.
Acquisition of data: Bellinger, Gillman, Oken.
Analysis and interpretation of data: Belfort,
Rifas-Shiman, Kleinman, Guthrie, Bellinger, Taveras,
Oken.
Drafting of the manuscript: Belfort.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important
intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Belfort, Rifas-Shiman, Kleinman,
Guthrie.
Obtained funding: Belfort, Gillman, Oken.
Administrative, technical, and material support:
Gillman.
Study supervision: Bellinger, Taveras, Gillman, Oken.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by
grants K23 DK083817 (Dr Belfort), K24 HL68041
(Dr Gillman), K24 HD069408 (Dr Oken), R01
ES016314, R01 HD34568, and R01 HL64925 from
the National Institutes of Health.
REFERENCES
1. Ip S, Chung M, Raman G, et al. Breastfeeding and
maternal and infant health outcomes in developed
countries. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep).
2007;(153):1-186.
2. Jain A, Concato J, Leventhal JM. How good is the
evidence linking breastfeeding and intelligence?
Pediatrics. 2002;109(6):1044-1053.
3. Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT.
Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a
meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 1999;70(4):525-535.
4. Drane DL, Logemann JA. A critical evaluation of
the evidence on the association between type of
infant feeding and cognitive development. Paediatr
Perinat Epidemiol. 2000;14(4):349-356.
5. Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Effect of breast
feeding on intelligence in children: prospective
study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. BMJ.
2006;333(7575):e945. http://
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1633819/.
Accessed June 15, 2013.
6. Jensen CL, Maude M, Anderson RE, Heird WC.
Effect of docosahexaenoic acid supplementation of
lactating women on the fatty acid composition of
breast milk lipids and maternal and infant plasma
phospholipids. Am J Clin Nutr. 2000;71(1)(suppl):
292S-299S.
7. Oken E, Radesky JS, Wright RO, et al. Maternal
fish intake during pregnancy, blood mercury levels,
and child cognition at age 3 years in a US cohort.
Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(10):1171-1181.
8. Oken E, Kleinman KP, Olsen SF, Rich-Edwards
JW, Gillman MW. Associations of seafood and
elongated n-3 fatty acid intake with fetal growth
and length of gestation: results from a US
pregnancy cohort. Am J Epidemiol.
2004;160(8):774-783.
9. Gillman MW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP,
Rich-Edwards JW, Lipshultz SE. Maternal calcium
intake and offspring blood pressure. Circulation.
2004;110(14):1990-1995.
10. Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Scanlon KS,
Grummer-Strawn LM, Sherry B, Gillman MW. To
what extent is the protective effect of
breastfeeding on future overweight explained by
decreased maternal feeding restriction? Pediatrics.
2006;118(6):2341-2348.
11. Williams KT, Wang J-J. Technical References to
the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition
(PPVT-III). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Service Inc; 1997.
12. Adams W, Sheslow D. WRAVMA (Wide Range
Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities). Wilmington,
DE: Wide Range Inc; 1995.
13. Grados JJ, Russo-Garcia KA. Comparison of the
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test and the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition in
economically disadvantaged African American
youth. J Clin Psychol. 1999;55(9):1063-1071.
14. Adams W, Sheslow D. Wide Range Assessment
of Memory and Learning Administration and
Technical Manual. 2nd ed. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources Inc; 2001.
15. Gillman MW, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman
SL, Lieberman ES, Kleinman KP, Lipshultz SE.
Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition Original Investigation Research
jamapediatrics.com JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 843
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
Maternal age and other predictors of newborn
blood pressure. J Pediatr. 2004;144(2):240-245.
16. Fawzi WW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Rich-Edwards JW,
Willett WC, Gillman MW. Calibration of a
semi-quantitative food frequency questionnaire in
early pregnancy. Ann Epidemiol. 2004;14(10):
754-762.
17. Frankenburg WK, Coons CE. Home Screening
Questionnaire: its validity in assessing home
environment. J Pediatr. 1986;108(4):624-626.
18. Oken E, Kleinman KP, Rich-Edwards J, Gillman
MW. A nearly continuous measure of birth weight
for gestational age using a United States national
reference. BMC Pediatr. 2003;3:e6.
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2431/3/6.
Accessed June 15, 2013.
19. Horton NJ, Kleinman KP. Much ado about
nothing: a comparison of missing data methods and
software to fit incomplete data regression models.
Am Stat. 2007;61(1):79-90.
20. Morrow-Tlucak M, Haude RH, Ernhart CB.
Breastfeeding and cognitive development in the
first 2 years of life. Soc Sci Med. 1988;26(6):
635-639.
21. Wigg NR, Tong S, McMichael AJ, Baghurst PA,
Vimpani G, Roberts R. Does breastfeeding at six
months predict cognitive development? Aust N Z J
Public Health. 1998;22(2):232-236.
22. Jacobson SW, Chiodo LM, Jacobson JL.
Breastfeeding effects on intelligence quotient in 4-
and 11-year-old children. Pediatrics.
1999;103(5):e71. http://www.biomedcentral.com
/1471-2431/3/6. Accessed June 15, 2013.
23. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Sharp D, Asten P, Mills A,
Kumar R. Intellectual problems shown by
11-year-old children whose mothers had postnatal
depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry.
2001;42(7):871-889.
24. Brion MJ, Lawlor DA, Matijasevich A, et al.
What are the causal effects of breastfeeding on IQ,
obesity and blood pressure? evidence from
comparing high-income with middle-income
cohorts. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(3):670-680.
25. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, et al;
Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial
(PROBIT) Study Group. Breastfeeding and child
cognitive development: new evidence from a large
randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry.
2008;65(5):578-584.
26. Brenna JT, Varamini B, Jensen RG,
Diersen-Schade DA, Boettcher JA, Arterburn LM.
Docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acid
concentrations in human breast milk worldwide.
Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85(6):1457-1464.
27. Sanders TA, Reddy S. The influence of a
vegetarian diet on the fatty acid composition of
human milk and the essential fatty acid status of the
infant. J Pediatr. 1992;120(4, pt 2):S71-S77.
28. Jensen CL, Voigt RG, Prager TC, et al. Effects of
maternal docosahexaenoic acid intake on visual
function and neurodevelopment in breastfed term
infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82(1):125-132.
29. Cheatham CL, Nerhammer AS, Asserhøj M,
Michaelsen KF, Lauritzen L. Fish oil
supplementation during lactation: effects on
cognition and behavior at 7 years of age. Lipids.
2011;46(7):637-645.
30. Campoy C, Escolano-Margarit MV, Anjos T,
Szajewska H, Uauy R. Omega 3 fatty acids on child
growth, visual acuity and neurodevelopment. Br J
Nutr. 2012;107(suppl 2):S85-S106.
31. Sattler JM. Assessment of Children: Cognitive
Applications. 4th ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M.
Sattler Inc; 2001.
Research Original Investigation Infant Feeding and Childhood Cognition
844 JAMA Pediatrics September 2013 Volume 167, Number 9 jamapediatrics.com
Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by Eliza Dean on 02/04/2016
Developed by Ron Lally and Cathy Tsao. © 2004, WestEd, The Program for Infant/Toddler Care.
This document may be reproduced for educational purposes.
Definition
The concept of continuity of care refers to the policy of assigning a primary caregiver to an
infant at the time of enrollment in a child care program and continuing this relationship until the
child is at least three years old.
With continuity, the infant is not moved to a new group; either the entire group moves with the
caregiver to another space more appropriate for older infants, or the caregiver modifies the
environment to meet the children’s changing needs. The most important relationship to
continue is the one between the child and the primary caregiver, but promoting long-term
relationships with other caregivers and with other children in the group is also important.
Foundation: Theory and Research
In attachment theory, stability and consistency of caregivers are seen as important factors in the
development and maintenance of secure attachment relationships (Bowlby, 1973). Children
take a significant amount of time to form attachments to caregivers, so they are less likely to
form attachments if frequent caregiver changes occur. More frequent changes in primary
caregiver have been associated with negative child outcomes, including withdrawing behaviors
and higher levels of aggression as preschoolers (Howes & Hamilton, 1993).
On the other hand, time with a high-ability teacher has been positively associated with security
of attachment in infant day care (Raikes, 1993). When infants are able to develop a relationship
with a teacher over time, the infant-teacher relationship becomes more defined and predictable,
creating a secure base for the infant. Raikes (1993) found that attachment security increased
as children spent more time with their primary caregivers, and that 91% of infants who had been
with their teacher for over one year were categorized as securely attached. These results are
consistent with attachment theory, which suggests that time in a relationship is required in order
for secure attachments to develop. However, the theory does not imply that time with an
insensitive or non-responsive teacher would also promote security in infant-teacher attachment;
in fact, such an arrangement may lead to the infant’s developing an insecure relationship with
the teacher. The quality with which the teacher interacts with the child is key.
Context
There is a clear link between child care quality and child outcomes, where child care quality is
defined by either structural variables, which can be regulated, such as group size and continuity
of care; or process variables, which require judgment and interpretation, and are difficult or
impossible to regulate, such as sensitive and responsive caregiving (Howes, Phillips, &
Whitebook, 1992). Structural variables are thought to influence process variables, and
researchers (Raikes, 1993) have conceptualized the infant-teacher relationship as a mediator
between child care quality and child outcomes.
Continuity of Care
2
Research and theory have established the necessity of intimate caregiving relationships for
laying a foundation of trust, providing predictability, regulating attention, and creating interest for
infants. Raikes (1993) hypothesizes that “time with teacher”—the length of time a high-ability
teacher cares for an infant—is an indicator of quality for the infant-teacher relationship.
Structural Variables
In the context of the PITC philosophy’s six essential policies1, continuity supports the secure,
warm relationships infants and toddlers need in order to thrive. Continuity begins with
assignment of a primary caregiver who develops a close relationship with the child and the
family and is primarily responsible for the child’s care. These relationships—and all
relationships in the setting—are supported by the organization of caregivers and children into
small groups. These three program policies—primary care, small groups, and continuity—
promote feelings of security and predictability for infants and toddlers in group care. These
policies work best together, but any one of them will improve the quality of care.
Process Variables
The ideal match is between program policies that support relationships and highly sensitive
teachers who implement those policies. In addition to the three policies described above, other
components of the PITC philosophy—responsive caregiving, culturally sensitive care and family
support, and attention to temperamental differences—contribute to the quality of interactions.
With a working knowledge of these components teachers become increasingly able to provide
appropriate infant care and family support.
Barriers
Among continuity, primary caregiving, and small groups, the concept of continuity is the most
likely to encounter resistance, in part because it is the newest idea. In a study of child care
centers across the United States, the practice of continuity of care was found to be rare (Cryer,
Hurwitz, & Wolery, 2000). While the infant’s need for close relationships is incontrovertible, the
idea that we can and should promote long-term relationships in infant/toddler programs
challenges us to new heights at a time when many programs are consumed with basic survival.
Teachers and administrators need support and information to help them in the process of
implementation of continuity of care and to ensure the entire staff believes in its role in the
development of relationships and healthy attachment. Without disregarding the real-life
concerns that programs face, we can strive to set the bar—rather than aim for the lowest
common denominator—by encouraging programs to take one step at a time and to move
steadily to the provision of continuity.
Structuring infant/toddler programs to promote long-term relationships often means reorganizing
the environment, staffing, training, admission policies—the very systems by which we staff,
enroll and care for children. It raises logistical questions that cannot be answered in the
abstract, e.g., safety regulations, subsidy requirements, age-related fee and reimbursement
schedules. Every program needs to find its own solutions to the arrangement of space and
equipment, the scheduling of children and caregivers, staffing ratios and group size, etc.
Ultimately, continuity of child care relationships—along with small groups and primary
caregiving assignments—benefit everyone in a program, promoting staff and family cooperation
1 PITC’s six essential policies are continuity, primary caregiving, small groups, individualized care,
cultural sensitivity, and inclusion of children with special needs.
3
and loyalty; creating a calmer, less stressful environment; and providing better opportunities for
the growth and development of adults as well as children.
References
Barnas, M.V., & Cummings, E.M. (1994). Caregiver stability and toddlers’ attachment-related
behavior towards caregivers in day care. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 141 –
147.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation. London: Basic Books.
Cryer, D., Hurwitz, S., & Wolery, M. (2000). Continuity of caregiver for infants and
toddlers in center-based child care: Report on a survey of center practices. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(4), 497 – 514.
Howes, C., & Hamilton, C.E. (1993). The changing experience of child care: Changes in
teachers and in teacher-child relationships and children’s social competence with peers.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 15 – 32.
Howes, C., Phillips, D.A., & Whitebook, M. (1992). Thresholds of quality: Implications for the
social development of children in center-based child care. Child Development, 63, 449 –
460.
Raikes, H. (1993). Relationship duration infant care: Time with a high-ability teacher and infant-
teacher attachment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 309 – 325.
<<
/ASCII85EncodePages false
/AllowTransparency false
/AutoPositionEPSFiles true
/AutoRotatePages /None
/Binding /Left
/CalGrayProfile (Dot Gain 20%)
/CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated \050SWOP\051 v2)
/sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1)
/CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Error
/CompatibilityLevel 1.4
/CompressObjects /Tags
/CompressPages true
/ConvertImagesToIndexed true
/PassThroughJPEGImages true
/CreateJobTicket false
/DefaultRenderingIntent /Default
/DetectBlends true
/DetectCurves 0.0000
/ColorConversionStrategy /CMYK
/DoThumbnails false
/EmbedAllFonts true
/EmbedOpenType false
/ParseICCProfilesInComments true
/EmbedJobOptions true
/DSCReportingLevel 0
/EmitDSCWarnings false
/EndPage -1
/ImageMemory 1048576
/LockDistillerParams false
/MaxSubsetPct 100
/Optimize true
/OPM 1
/ParseDSCComments true
/ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true
/PreserveCopyPage true
/PreserveDICMYKValues true
/PreserveEPSInfo true
/PreserveFlatness true
/PreserveHalftoneInfo false
/PreserveOPIComments true
/PreserveOverprintSettings true
/StartPage 1
/SubsetFonts true
/TransferFunctionInfo /Apply
/UCRandBGInfo /Preserve
/UsePrologue false
/ColorSettingsFile ()
/AlwaysEmbed [ true
]
/NeverEmbed [ true
]
/AntiAliasColorImages false
/CropColorImages true
/ColorImageMinResolution 300
/ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleColorImages true
/ColorImageDownsampleType /Bicubic
/ColorImageResolution 300
/ColorImageDepth -1
/ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1
/ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000
/EncodeColorImages true
/ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterColorImages true
/ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/ColorACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/ColorImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000ColorImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasGrayImages false
/CropGrayImages true
/GrayImageMinResolution 300
/GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleGrayImages true
/GrayImageDownsampleType /Bicubic
/GrayImageResolution 300
/GrayImageDepth -1
/GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2
/GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000
/EncodeGrayImages true
/GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode
/AutoFilterGrayImages true
/GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG
/GrayACSImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/GrayImageDict <<
/QFactor 0.15
/HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1]
>>
/JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/JPEG2000GrayImageDict <<
/TileWidth 256
/TileHeight 256
/Quality 30
>>
/AntiAliasMonoImages false
/CropMonoImages true
/MonoImageMinResolution 1200
/MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK
/DownsampleMonoImages true
/MonoImageDownsampleType /Bicubic
/MonoImageResolution 1200
/MonoImageDepth -1
/MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000
/EncodeMonoImages true
/MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode
/MonoImageDict <<
/K -1
>>
/AllowPSXObjects false
/CheckCompliance [
/None
]
/PDFX1aCheck false
/PDFX3Check false
/PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false
/PDFXNoTrimBoxError true
/PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox true
/PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
0.00000
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfile ()
/PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier ()
/PDFXOutputCondition ()
/PDFXRegistryName ()
/PDFXTrapped /False
/CreateJDFFile false
/Description <<
/ARA
/BGR
/CHS
/CHT
/CZE
/DAN
/DEU
/ESP
/ETI
/FRA
/GRE
/HEB
/HRV (Za stvaranje Adobe PDF dokumenata najpogodnijih za visokokvalitetni ispis prije tiskanja koristite ove postavke. Stvoreni PDF dokumenti mogu se otvoriti Acrobat i Adobe Reader 5.0 i kasnijim verzijama.)
/HUN
/ITA
/JPN
/KOR
/LTH
/LVI
/NLD (Gebruik deze instellingen om Adobe PDF-documenten te maken die zijn geoptimaliseerd voor prepress-afdrukken van hoge kwaliteit. De gemaakte PDF-documenten kunnen worden geopend met Acrobat en Adobe Reader 5.0 en hoger.)
/NOR
/POL
/PTB
/RUM
/RUS
/SKY
/SLV
/SUO
/SVE
/TUR
/UKR
/ENU (Use these settings to create Adobe PDF documents best suited for high-quality prepress printing. Created PDF documents can be opened with Acrobat and Adobe Reader 5.0 and later.)
>>
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(Common)
(1.0)
]
/OtherNamespaces [
<<
/AsReaderSpreads false
/CropImagesToFrames true
/ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue
/FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false
/IncludeGuidesGrids false
/IncludeNonPrinting false
/IncludeSlug false
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(InDesign)
(4.0)
]
/OmitPlacedBitmaps false
/OmitPlacedEPS false
/OmitPlacedPDF false
/SimulateOverprint /Legacy
>>
<<
/AddBleedMarks false
/AddColorBars false
/AddCropMarks false
/AddPageInfo false
/AddRegMarks false
/ConvertColors /ConvertToCMYK
/DestinationProfileName ()
/DestinationProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK
/Downsample16BitImages true
/FlattenerPreset <<
/PresetSelector /MediumResolution
>>
/FormElements false
/GenerateStructure false
/IncludeBookmarks false
/IncludeHyperlinks false
/IncludeInteractive false
/IncludeLayers false
/IncludeProfiles false
/MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings
/Namespace [
(Adobe)
(CreativeSuite)
(2.0)
]
/PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK
/PreserveEditing true
/UntaggedCMYKHandling /LeaveUntagged
/UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile
/UseDocumentBleed false
>>
]
>> setdistillerparams
<<
/HWResolution [2400 2400]
/PageSize [612.000 792.000]
>> setpagedevice
Clinical digest
NEW EVIDENCE ADDS TO
THE BREAST VERSUS
BOTTLE-FEEDING DEBATE
UK research compares temperaments
of babies fed by different methods
New mothers should be informed that it is
normal for breastfed babies to cry more and
appear less content than bottle-fed babies.
The Medical Research Council asked mothers
to assess the temperaments of their babies at
three months using a behaviour questionnaire.
Of the 316 babies, 137 were exclusively
breastfed, 88 were fed with formula milk and
91 were fed with a combination.
Overall, breast and mixed-fed babies were
rated by their mothers as having more
challenging temperaments. Compared to
formula-fed babies, breastfed babies showed
greater distress, and were less likely to smile,
laugh or vocalise. They were also slower to calm
down following distress or excitement.
Lead researcher Ken Ong, a paediatrician from
the Medical Research Council epidemiology unit
in Cambridge, says: ‘Bottle-fed babies may
appear more content, but research suggests
that they could gain weight too quickly.
‘Rather than being put off breastfeeding,
parents should have more realistic expectations
of normal infant
behaviour and should
receive better
support to cope
with difficult infant
behaviours if needed.’
The Department of
Health recommends
breastfeeding for the
first six months, but most
UK mothers switch to
bottle-feeding within
four months.
de Lauzon-Guillain
B et al (2012)
Breastfeeding
and infant
temperament
at age three
months.
PLoS One. doi:
10.1371/journal.
pone.0029326.
says more large studies are
needed to establish whether
red meat is a risk factor for
pancreatic cancer.
She adds: ‘Stopping smoking
is the best way to reduce your
chances of developing many
types of cancer – and other
diseases too.’
Larsson SC et al (2012) Red and
processed meat consumption
and risk of pancreatic cancer:
meta-analysis of prospective studies.
British Journal of Cancer.
doi: 10.1038/bjc.2011.585.
Bariatric surgery could have
more pronounced ‘benefits’
than standard lifestyle changes
Weight-loss surgery reduces the
risk of heart attacks and other
cardiovascular events, according
to research conducted as part
of the Swedish Obese Subjects
(SOS) study.
SOS is an ongoing study
of 2,010 participants who
underwent bariatric surgery
and a control group of 2,037
individuals who received usual
(non-surgical) care for their
obesity. Patients were aged
37 to 60, with a body mass
index of at least 34 in men and
at least 38 in women.
They were recruited between
1987 and 2001 and there was
a mean follow-up of 14.7 years.
Surgery patients underwent
vertical banded gastroplasty
(68.1 per cent), gastric bypass
(13.2 per cent) and banding
(18.7 per cent).
At two years follow-up, the
mean change in body weight in
surgery patients was a decrease
of 23 per cent; at ten years the
decrease was 16 per cent, at
20 years it was 18 per cent. In
the control group, there was no
change in body weight at two
years follow-up and only a 1 per
cent decrease at 15 and 20 years.
Swedish research establishes
link between eating processed
meat and pancreatic cancer
Eating processed meat increases
the risk of pancreatic cancer,
suggests a study that also found
red meat increases the risk of
the cancer in men.
Researchers at the Karolinksa
Institutet in Stockholm, Sweden,
conducted a meta-analysis of
11 studies involving 6,643
pancreatic cancer cases. They
found that for each 50 grams of
processed meat eaten each day –
equivalent to one sausage or two
rashers of bacon – there was a
19 per cent rise in the risk of
pancreatic cancer compared
to those who ate no meat.
Consuming 150g of processed
meat per day increased the risk
by 58 per cent.
The evidence for red meat
was inconclusive, with an
increase in risk for men, but
not for women. Men who ate
120g per day of red meat had
a 29 per cent increased risk of
pancreatic cancer compared
to those who ate no meat.
Study author Susanna Larsson
says that pancreatic cancer’s
poor survival rate means it is
important to understand the
factors that increase the risk
of the disease. ‘If diet affects
pancreatic cancer then this
could influence public health
campaigns to help reduce the
number of cases of this disease
developing in the first place.’
Cancer Research UK
information director Sara Hiom
NURSING STANDARD
A
LA
M
Y
IS
TO
C
K
PH
O
TO
p16-17w22_CLINICAL DIGEST this one copy 5 30/01/2012 12:13 Page 16
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PEDIATRIC REVIEW
The role of responsive feeding in overweight during
infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic
review
KI DiSantis1, EA Hodges2, SL Johnson3 and JO Fisher
4
1Center for Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA;
2Family Health Division, School of Nursing, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA;
3Department of Pediatrics, Section of Nutrition, University of Colorado-Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, CO, USA and
4Department of Public Health, Temple University, Center for Obesity Research and Education, Philadelphia, PA, USA
A chronic mismatch of caregiver responsiveness to infant-feeding cues, such as feeding when the infant is not hungry, is
hypothesized to have a role in the development of overweight by impairing an infant’s response to internal states of hunger and
satiation. Although this concept of mismatch or discordance has long been acknowledged in scholarly writings, a systematic
assessment of the evidence supporting the role of discordant responsiveness during infant feeding in the early origins of
overweight is lacking. This review was undertaken to assess evidence for this hypothesized relationship between discordant
responsiveness in feeding and overweight in infancy and toddlerhood, framed within the larger social-environmental context of
the infant–caregiver dyad. A systematic method was used to extract articles from three databases of the medical, psychology
and nursing fields. The quality of evidence collected was assessed using Oxford University Centre for Evidence Based Medicine’s
level of evidence and through a narrative review. The systematic search resulted in only nine original research studies, which met
a priori inclusion/exclusion criteria. Several studies provide support for the conceptual model, but most were cross-sectional or
lower quality prospective studies. The need for consistent definitions, improved measures and longitudinal work is discussed.
In conclusion, this review reveals preliminary support for the proposed role of discordant responsiveness in infant/child overweight
and at the same time highlights the need for rigorous investigation of responsive feeding interactions in the first years of life.
International Journal of Obesity
(2011) 35, 480–492; doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.3; published online 22 March 2011
Keywords: infant; feeding behavior; responsiveness; appetite regulation; self-regulation; overweight
Introduction
Infancy is believed to be a sensitive period for the develop-
ment of energy intake regulation1 and overweight.2–6 In the
first 2 years of life, infants and toddlers are dependent on
their caregivers to provide adequate and appropriate nutri-
tion. For this reason, the potential influence of feeding
dynamics on overnutrition and the development of over-
weight is seemingly intuitive. Scholarly work on early life
origins of overweight, however, has largely ignored the
possible impacts of social and behavioral transactions on
infant feeding. We focus on one particular aspect of these
caregiver–infant transactions: responsive feeding. In this
review, we define responsiveness within the context of
overweight, as involving prompt, contingent and develop-
mentally appropriate responses to the infant’s hunger and
satiety cues. As the review will describe, the degree to which
feeding interactions are responsive is believed to hold
importance in infant development by nurturing or impeding
the development of self-regulation. Self-regulation, generally
defined as ‘the ability to regulate reactionsyand to function
more independently in a personal and social context, (pg 93)’
is said to have multi-dimensional influence on functioning,
including behavioral, emotional and cognitive actions.7 The
early development of this general capacity is profoundly
affected by supportive caregiver–infant interactions and it is
likely that these interactions have a similar impact on the
development of eating self-regulation. We present a model in
which a caregiver’s responsiveness that is congruent with
infant-feeding cues encourages the development of infant self-
regulation as it relates to energy intake, and eventually obesity.
Overweight and accelerated weight gain in infancy
and toddlerhood
The problem of pediatric overweight is evident before the
preschool years. Data collected in the National Health and
Received 21 June 2010; revised 6 December 2010; accepted 26 December
2010; published online 22 March 2011
Correspondence: Dr KI DiSantis, School of Medicine, Center for Clinical
Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of Pennsylvania, 8th floor Blockley
Hall, 423 Guardian drive, Philadelphia PA 19104, USA.
E-mail: disantis@med.mail.upenn.edu
International Journal of Obesity (2011) 35, 480–
492
& 2011 Macmillan Publishers Limited All rights reserved 0307-0565/11
www.nature.com/ijo
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2011.3
mailto:disantis@med.mail.upenn.edu
http://www.nature.com/ijo
Nutrition Examination Survey 2007–2008 indicated that
approximately 10% of infants and toddlers were above the
95th percentile for weight-for-length; the prevalence was as
high as 14.9% in Hispanic males.8 These numbers are
of concern because heavy infants are at increased risk of
overweight in later stages of development.9,10 Analysis of the
CDC Pediatric Nutrition Surveillance System data revealed
that overweight infants (0–11 months) were 2.9–4.3 times
more likely to be overweight between the ages of 1 and 4
years than non-overweight infants.11 Accelerated weight
gain during the first 2 years of life, independent of birth
weight and parental weight status, has also been associated
with a greater risk of overweight during childhood12–17 and
young adulthood4,18–21 (see Stettler and Iotova22 for a
review). Though discussion of etiology has favored biological
explanations, caregiver–infant feeding interaction, like re-
sponsiveness, is implicit when considering the potential
contributing factors of infant weight gain.14,23
Caregiving and self-regulation among infants and
toddlers
Why might caregiver responsiveness be a formative dimen-
sion of feeding during the first 2 years of life?
Responsiveness
is a dimension of infant–caregiver interactions, which has
been said to shape an infant’s ability to acquire self-
regulation.7 Early relationships with caregivers engender an
infant’s development in numerous ways, but a supportive
environment to enhance the development of self-regulation
of energy intake holds unique importance for childhood
obesity risk. Supporting infant self-regulation of energy
intake may provide the necessary conditions to facilitate
development of regulatory capacity and autonomy through-
out infancy and into childhood. It is believed that infants
and young children are born with a nascent capacity for self-
regulation that becomes actualized through cause–effect
learning, which occurs when their behavior is consistently
met with a prompt, developmentally appropriate response.24
Responsive caregiving has been studied for decades in
domains outside of child feeding and growth, and has been
positively associated with a wide range of developmental
outcomes in infancy and early childhood including
emotional, language and cognitive capacity, as well as the
security of attachment to the mother (see Ziv et al.25 for a
review). Such synchronous interactions are thought to
provide fundamental support for opportunities to learn
mastery and self-regulation.26 Early infant emotions, for
example, stem from physical states, including temperature,
sleep states and hunger.7 Newborns exhibit emotional
reactivity to these physical states and express discomfort
via crying, but begin to show signs of self-regulated behavior
by 3 months of age.27 Examples of self-regulated behavior
include turning the body or head away from undesirable
stimuli and moving the hands toward the head and mouth.7
Caregivers support this developmental progression from
emotional reactivity to self-regulation by their consistent,
accurate and appropriate responses to infant cues, which
foster expectations in the developing infant about predict-
able patterns of interaction.7 However, accurate interpreta-
tion by caregivers is complicated by the fact that infant
crying can reflect discomfort due a variety of physical states
including sleep, temperature and hunger states.28 Thus, it
should be acknowledged that caregiver and child are
mutually influencing one another over time through the
consistency, meaning and appropriate interpretation of one
another’s behaviors.
Caregiver influences on the infant’s development of
self-regulation of feeding behavior parallels emotional devel-
opment. In their monthly feeding observations of 26 mother–
infant pairs over the first year of life, Ainsworth and Bell29
observed that mothers who exhibited more sensitive pacing
and prompt responses to the infant during feeding had infants
who cried less in early infancy and demonstrated greater
attachment to their mothers at the end of the first year,
compared with mothers who adhered to rigid feeding
schedules. This supports the hypothesis that caregiver respon-
sivity affects infant outcomes and development.30,31 However,
the relationship between caregiver and infant is bidirectional
and can also be affected by the clarity of infant cues.32,33 Some
infants demonstrate ambiguous signals, and have fewer
behaviors in general, including fewer elicited and spontaneous
vocalizations, fewer smiles, less gaze and joint attention and
poor signals to indicate their needs (including hunger cues). In
such cases, a caregiver’s responsiveness might be discordant
with infant needs (due to poor clarity of cues) or might be a
low level because few cues are exhibited.
To this notion, Wright and colleagues34,35 have argued
that appropriate responses to infant-feeding cues are critical
for self-regulation and self-control of food intake to develop
to its fullest potential. This argument is evidenced by
experimental studies, which have demonstrated that infants
as young as 6 weeks of age could adjust the volume of
formula intake in response to its energy content to maintain
daily energy intake.36–38 Observational research has also
provided evidence of such a capacity, including data from
3022 children (6 to 24 month old ) participating in the
Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study showing inverse relation-
ships between the number of daily feeds and the size of
feeds.39 In the context of developmental literature on
responsive parenting, self-regulation of energy intake in
infancy and toddlerhood is viewed as the extent to which
feeding environments and interactions allow the biological
potential of the child for self-regulation to be actualized.40
A potential role for responsive feeding
Although contribution of responsive feeding to self-regula-
tion and overweight in early development has been
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
481
International Journal of Obesity
suggested in scholarly writings for decades,35,41–44 systematic
inquiry appears limited. This review was undertaken to
characterize empirical support for the assertion that varia-
tion in the concordance of a caregiver’s responsiveness and
an infant’s hunger and satiety cues has a fundamentally
important role in growth by supporting energy self-regula-
tion in infancy and toddlerhood. Figure 1 presents the
conceptual framework that guided the review by articulating
potential pathways through which caregiver responsiveness
to infant-feeding cues influences child intake and growth.
Drawing from the developmental literature, feeding respon-
siveness is defined for the purposes of this review as prompt,
contingent and developmentally appropriate responses to
the child’s hunger and satiety cues.24,45 The bidirectional
nature of feeding is implicit in this definition, with the
caregiver dependent on the infant to provide clear, un-
ambiguous cues and the infant dependent on the caregiver’s
accurate interpretation of and appropriate response to those
cues for obtaining adequate and appropriate nutrition.
Given the aforementioned variance in the clarity of infant-
feeding cues, caregivers may be challenged to a lesser or
greater degree in their interpretation of cues. Likewise, less-
responsive caregivers may challenge the infant to provide
clearer cues (within a repertoire that may be relatively
limited in the first months of life). Thus, when we refer to
caregiver feeding responsiveness, we are referring to a
characteristic of the caregiver–child dyad’s level of concor-
dance in interactions during feeding. Responsiveness to
child-feeding cues has previously been included as part of an
internationally recognized framework of infant-care
practices oriented toward the prevention of malnutrition
and the support of child mental, social and physical
development.31,44,46 In that work, responsive feeding has
been broadly conceptualized to involve the caregiver’s
adaptation to the child’s psychomotor abilities for feeding
and provision of a structured feeding environment with a
goal of facilitating energy consumption to offset the effects
of malnutrition.44 In contrast, this review focuses solely on
the role of caregiver responsiveness to feeding cues in
supporting the capacity of infants to self-regulate energy
intake for optimal growth in a context of obesity risk.
Figure 1 was developed drawing from scholarly writing
dating back almost a half century.35,41–44 In this model,
discordant caregiver responsiveness to infant cues, specifi-
cally feeding without hunger and feeding beyond satiety, is
hypothesized to impair infant-satiety response. Such impair-
ments are suggested to promote energy intake beyond needs
for growth via increases in meal size and/or feeding
frequency. Overweight and rapid weight gain during infancy
and toddlerhood are the main growth-related outcomes of
interest. This model acknowledges feeding responsiveness as
being nested within a broader social-environmental context
that influences the way in which caregivers feed their
children,47 including cultural influences (e.g., ethnicity/
race48), policy and prevailing feeding guidance (e.g., World
Health Organization Recommendations on Breastfeeding49),
parental beliefs/goals/values,50 and parental knowledge of
development.51 These are all the factors brought in to the
feeding relationshipFand all can have considerable effects
on the relationship. This model acknowledges these ele-
ments, which are at the foundation of building a responsive
feeding relationship. The review was initiated to assess the
amount and quality of empirical evidence available to
support this
proposed model.
Materials and methods
Literature-search strategy
We performed a systematic literature review of responsive
feeding using the three major databases from three dis-
ciplines (medicine, psychology and nursing): MEDLINE via
PubMed (United States National Library of Medicine,
Bethesda, MD, USA), PsycINFO (American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC, USA) and CINAHL (Ebscohost,
Glendale, CA, USA). These databases were searched for
articles listed from the database’s inception to September
2009. Database limits were used to restrict the search to
research in humans and infants (from birth to 24 months).
As listed in Table 1, a list of 25 search terms were generated
by the research team to identify relevant articles addressing
Accelerated Weight Gain/
Overweight
Accelerated Weight Gain/
Overweight
Infant
Caregiver
Clarity of Hunger
and Satiety Cues
Discordant
Feeding
Responsiveness
Discordant
Feeding
Responsiveness
Increased Feeding
Frequency/Amount
Increased Feeding
Frequency/Amount
Accurate Interpretation
Developmentally
Appropriate Response
Awareness
Of Feeding Cues
Impaired Self-Regulation
•Diminished satiation/satiety
•Increased energy intake
Impaired Self-Regulation
•Diminished satiation/satiety
•Increased energy intake
Figure 1 Figure 1 presents a model for the pathway from discordant feeding
responsiveness to accelerated weight gain and/or overweight. This model
suggests that chronic mismatch between a caregiver’s responsiveness and an
infant’s feeding cues can result in increased feedings (amount and/or
frequency), which eventually lead to the acceleration of weight gain and
overweight via impaired infant self-regulation. A caregiver’s responsiveness
refers to both the amount and quality of responsive behaviors. The model
illustrates that caregiver and infant relationship is reciprocal, which is
impacted by the clarity of infant cues. The model also acknowledges that
the caregiver–infant dyad exists within a socio-environmental context, which
might impact the relationship.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
482
International Journal of Obesity
the concepts outlined in the conceptual model (Figure 1),
including responsive feeding, self-regulation, feeding/
hunger cues, as well as terms related to obesity, such as rapid
weight gain. The phrases/terms in quotations were searched
for the exact phrase/term, whereas the other terms were not
put in quotations to allow for a variety of combination terms
to be searched. All search terms that included the word ‘Infant’
were repeated using the term ‘Child’ to ensure an extensive
search of the infant population.
Study selection and
grading
Article titles and their abstracts were briefly reviewed by the
first author (KI DiSantis) using the selection criteria. Those
that met and questionably met the inclusion/exclusion
criteria were then thoroughly reviewed for eligibility by all
the authors. Inclusion criteria included the following:
original research, published in English, published up to
September 2009, involving healthy full-term infants, aged
0–24 months of child age, including infant feeding/eating
and/or infant weight/growth outcomes, and oriented toward
child overweight/rapid weight gain and/or obesity. Owing to
concerns about socioeconomic and cultural consistency
across studies, and the goal of focusing on overnutrition
and obesity (as opposed to slow growth and malnutrition),
exclusion criteria included research conducted in developing
countries. In addition, given the focus on infant overweight
and obesity, research that focused on growth faltering
(organic or non-organic) or low birth weight was excluded.
Although including growth faltering and malnutrition
studies would increase the quantity of articles extracted,
they likely would not have added to the content of this
review as responsive feeding in these studies are investigated
in relation to a very different set of outcomes, including
promotion of energy intake and weight gain.
The quality assessment phase measured the level of
evidence provided by each included study and utilized the
Oxford University Centre for Evidence Based Medicine
(CEBM) levels of evidence system.52 This system was
developed to be an advancement of the Canadian Task Force
on the Periodic Health Examination grading system of 1979.
The ‘level’ of evidence is graded, primarily based on the rigor
of the study design (e.g., randomization in interventions),
sampling (e.g., response rate), longitudinal versus cross-
sectional, outcome assessment (e.g., negative assessment if
outcome measure is imprecise) and the follow-up rate, with
‘1a’ being the highest and ‘5’ being the lowest level. Levels of
evidence were independently assigned to the included
papers by two of the study authors. A consensus approach
was used to resolve discordant assessments, in which a third
reviewer independently graded the article and discussion by
all authors was used to assign a final rating.
Results
Systematic review results
The results of the systematic review are detailed in Table 1
and illustrated in Figure 2. The search of three databases with
25 aforementioned keywords provided 1877 hits. Owing to
the fact that these databases contain subscriptions to the
same journals or indexes, it was not possible to derive a
unique number of hits. After elimination of redundant
abstracts, 82 unique abstracts were deemed appropriate for
full-abstract review based on the first author’s brief review of
article titles and abstracts. These abstracts were then
thoroughly reviewed by all authors based on the a priori
inclusion/exclusion criteria described above to determine
whether they would be selected for final analysis. First, 14
articles were excluded because they were not original
research studies, performed research in a population other
than infants of 0–24 months and/or the research was
performed with regard to malnutrition/growth faltering
(Figure 2). The 68 remaining articles were assessed to
determine whether some aspect of infant/child obesity was
directly measured (eating behavior, intake weight and/or
growth) and whether caregiver-feeding practices/behaviors
were assessed (Figure 2). A total of 29 articles were excluded
because some aspect of infant overweight/obesity or infant
eating/intake outcomes was not directly measured. In most
of these excluded cases, feeding observations or other
feeding measures were used as a tool to investigate general
parental responsiveness or general infant–caregiver
Table 1 Systematic review search results by search term
Search terms Number
of hits
from three
databases
Abstracts
selected for
detailed
review
Abstracts
selected
as meeting
criteria
for CEBM
grading
Responsive feeding 6 5 0
Caregiver feeding styles 6 2 0
Rapid weight gain and eating 6 2 0
Infant feeding and responsiveness 85 13 0
Child feeding and responsiveness 43 0 0
Self-regulation and eating 19 3 0
Infant-feeding cue or infant-feeding cues 13 1 0
Parent feeding styles 12 1 0
Rapid weight gain and Feeding 24 3 1
Feeding interactions 94 13 1
Infant feeding practices 328 9 2
Feeding and satiety response 7 2 1
Infant and satiety response 8 4 0
‘Infant growth’ and overweight 107 2 0
‘Infant feeding’ and obesity 99 3 2
Child and hunger cues 10 0 0
Child and fullness cues 1 0 0
Infant and hunger cues 20 2 0
Infant and fullness cues 1 0 0
Satiety cues and infant 12 2 1
Satiety cues and child 3 0 0
Internal cues and infant 26 1 0
Internal cues and child 18 0 0
Self-regulation and infant nutrition 41 5 1
‘Care for development’ and feeding 888 9 0
Total 1877 82 9
Abbreviation: CEBM, Oxford University Centre for Evidence Based Medicine.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
483
International Journal of Obesity
interaction, but the observations were never presented in the
context of infant eating/intake outcomes or were not
directed toward obesity outcomes child overweight/obesity.
Overall, 30 articles were excluded because of lack of
assessment of caregiver-feeding practices/behaviors, for
instance, in which feeding characteristics and infant growth
might have been assessed but no aspect of responsiveness
was measured. After screening all abstracts, a total of nine
articles met the inclusion/exclusion criteria and were
included in the systematic review (Figure 2).
The results of evidence-level grading for the nine articles,
based on CEBM criteria, are provided in Table 2. All articles
were graded as being a ‘2c’ level or lower level, meaning the
design was cross-sectional or a lower quality cohort study or
case–control study. Table 2 also provides additional details
related to the quality of the research studies, including
sampling characteristics, methods of measuring feeding
responsiveness and a brief outcome summary. Below we
briefly review these articles and their implications following
the framework of the conceptual model in Figure 1.
Narrative review
Socioenvironmental context of discordant responsiveness. Feed-
ing interactions take place within a wider social-environ-
mental context, consisting of a wide range of factors like
biobehavioral issues, culture, psychosocial status, physical
environments and history at individual, family and com-
munity level. Mogan53 was the only article in the review that
considered context, by evaluating associations of both
parental and infant weight status with maternal sensitivity
to infant cues. Parental weight status is an individual-
level
variable, but in this study they accounted for the differences
between dual and caregiver households with varying combi-
nations of parents classified as normal or overweight.
Parental weight status was categorized into three groups:
(1) two normal weight parents, (2) one overweight and one
normal and (3) two overweight parents. Mothers and infants
were observed during six feeding sessions from age 0 to 6
months to assess feeding interactions. The observations were
coded using the Nursing
Child Feeding Assessment Scale,
which contained the following subscales: mother’s sensitiv-
ity to cues, response to distress, social –and emotional
growth fostering, cognitive fostering and the infant’s clarity
of cues and responsiveness to their mother. This measure,
particularly the mother’s sensitivity to cues and infant
responsiveness subscales, allowed for the assessment of
discordance in responsiveness in a bidirectional manner as
presented in Figure 1. Although the methodology was
strong, the authors did not find that these responsive
feeding behaviors of the mother and/or infant differed
among the parental weight groups. However, infant weight
status differed significantly at 6 months of age according to
parent weight group, with infants who had one or two
overweight parents being more likely to be at or above the
90th percentile for weight. Mogan53 did not report on
relationships between maternal feeding responsiveness and
infant weight status at 6 months, so it is unclear whether any
relationship existed in this sample. Although this study used
observational data, which assessed the bidirectional feeding
relationship, it neither found relationship between parental
weight status and responsive maternal–infant feeding inter-
actions nor did it report on the association of responsive
feeding with infant weight at 6 months. Other studies of
older children have connected maternal weight status with
unresponsive feeding practices (i.e., restriction in feeding),54
but are outside the developmental scope of this review.
Discordant responsiveness and feeding frequency and amount. As
Figure 1 illustrates, discordant responsiveness in feeding
might lead to increased feeding frequency or amount.
Numerous circumstances of discordance could combine to
result in increased feeding frequency (including feeding an
infant in the absence of hunger cues, misperception of
hunger cues) and increased amounts of food (including
ignoring fullness cues, or misperception of fullness cues). If
this were to continue over a period of time, the risk of
increased energy would increase. Kavanagh et al.55 per-
formed a double-blinded, randomized intervention, which
focused on using education on feeding responsiveness and
prevention of overfeeding to reduce the risk of overfeeding
(infants were 3–10 weeks at enrollment). The control group
82 studies identified for full abstract review
4 excluded due to not being
original research
8 excluded due to sample being
malnourished, premature, or
having other health problem
involved in eating and/or weight
68 studies performed in samples
with healthy infants 0-24
months
9 original research studies performed in samples
with healthy infants 0-24 months with
obesity
outcomes and measuring and/or feeding and eating
included in evidence-based systematic review
78 studies original research
76 studies performed in
sample aged 0-24 months
39 studies performed in samples
with healthy infants 0-24 months
with obesity outcomes
29 excluded due to outcome not
being a direct measurement of
obesity
30 excluded child eating or
maternal feeding was not either
predictor or outcome
2 excluded due to sample age
Figure 2 Flow diagram of article extraction.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
484
International Journal of Obesity
Table 2 Systematic review results
Article Design, setting and variables Sample
characteristics
Measure of responsiveness
(type, description)
Summary of results CEBM
level
assigned
Social-environmental context of discordant responsiveness
Mogan53 Design: prospective cohort
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
Parents’ weight status (1 parent
overweight, 2 parents overweight,
2 parents normal weight)
Outcome(s): parent–child
interaction during six feedings
from 0 to 6 months and infant
weight status.
n¼78 for feeding
behavior
analysis
n¼62 for infant
weight status
analysis
Age: 55–70 h
Age(s) at
follow-up:
0–6 months
Race/ethnicity:
88.5% White,
5.1% Asian, 2.6%
East Indian, 1.3%
Black and 2.6%
other.
Type: observational measure
Description of measure: Nursing
Child Feeding Assessment Scale,
subscales: maternal sensitivity to
cues, clarity of child cues, child’s
responsiveness to parent during
feeds.
Feeding behaviors did not differ
among the parental weight groups
but a higher proportion of infants
with two overweight parents
had a growth percentile X90th
percentile
at
6 months.
2c
Discordant responsiveness and increased feeding frequency/amount
Kavanagh
et al.55
Design: double-blind, RCT
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
educational intervention with
general feeding guidelines and
information on awareness of
infant-satiety cues (single, 45-min
session); Control participants
received general feeding
guidelines only.
Outcome(s): formula intake
(ml h�1; non-weighed, 2-day
record) and weight gain.
n¼61 at BSL
n¼38 at follow-up
Age: 3–10 weeks
Age(s) at follow-
up: 4–5 months
Race/ethnicity: not
reported.
Type: Educational intervention
Description of measure:
intervention group received
feeding responsiveness
education, in addition to
general infant-feeding
guidelines.
The intervention group did not
differ significantly in formula intake
or weight gain in the hypothesized
direction, as the control group
reported lower formula intake
at 4–5 months and the intervention
group had greater weight gain
(grams per week).
2b
Discordant responsiveness and impaired self-regulation
Rybski et al.56 Design: cross-sectional
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/
intervention: maternal behaviors
during bottle feedings during six
feedings over a 24-h periods
Outcome(s): Infant-eating
behaviors (total feeding time,
sucking behaviors) and Infant
energy intake (24-h weighed
record).
n¼10
Age: 72 h
Race/ethnicity:
100% White.
Type: Observational measure
Description of measure:
observed maternal behaviors
during feedings (verbal
interaction, eye contact,
tender and caretaking
touching).
No association between the
measured maternal feeding
behaviors and either infant
eating behaviors or 24-hour
energy intake, but it is important
to consider that mothers were
not allowed to hold their infants
during any feeds, which would
clearly limit the amount of
behaviors a mother can perform.
4
Discordant responsiveness and infant weight gain/overweight.
Baughcuma
et al.70 Design: cross-sectional
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
child overweight (o90th percentile)
and mother’s obese status
(BMI X30)
Outcome(s): infant-feeding
styles.
n¼453
Age: 11–24
months
Race/ethnicity:
77% non-Hispanic
White, 16.6% non-
Hispanic Black,
1.4% Hispanic
White and 5%
others
Type: Self-report survey
Description of measure: Infant
Feeding Styles Questionnaire
(IFQ), subscales: Awareness of
infant’s hunger and satiety
cues, Feeding infant on
schedule, using food to calm
infant’s fussiness, social
interaction with infant
during feeding
The IFQ subscales failed to associate
responsive feeding behaviors to
concurrent weight status of either
infant (11–23 months) or mother.
2c
Dubois et al.69 Design: case–control
Setting (country): Canada
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
Infant weight status (Obese cases
(o90th percentile), normal controls
(25th–75th percentile))
Outcome(s): energy intake (3-day
non-weighed, record) and
maternal-feeding practices
n¼89 (47 cases,
42 controls)
Age: 4–9 months
Race/ethnicity: not
reported.
Type: qualitative self-report
Description of measure:
Open-ended questions on
mother’s past feeding practices
(e.g., breastfeeding) and
mother’s concepts and
attitudes toward infant
feeding.
Maternal responsiveness was an
identified theme. Obese and control
infants did not differ in the following
maternal responsiveness to feeding,
energy intake, or in her timing of
breastfeeding cessation or
supplementation introduction.
3b
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
485
International Journal of Obesity
(n¼ 21) received a 45-min educational session on general
guidelines for infant feeding, including appropriate age of
introduction of complementary foods, safe preparation of
complementary foods, responsive feeding practices, when
feeding complementary foods, and information on low-cost
ways of providing nutritionally balanced meals. The inter-
vention group (n¼19) was given similar information but
additionally provided with educational information on
being aware of infant-satiety cues when breastfeeding or
bottle feeding (e.g., understanding early versus late cues) and
were encouraged to only prepare 6 ounces of formula per
feeding. The outcome measures were both infant weight and
length and formula intake (measured at baseline, 2 weeks
after the class and at about 4 months. Kavanagh et al.55
found no differences between the intervention and control
groups with regard to formula intake at any of the time
points and bottle-emptying behavior, and conversely the
intervention group infants were heavier and taller at 4
months. Thus, this study did not provide support for the
conceptual model. Kavanagh et al.55 acknowledged short-
comings in this small study including that at baseline
the intervention group infant’s were taller and heavier than
the control group, the bottle records were not weighed
and a considerable loss to follow-up rate with only 38%
Table 2 (continued)
Article Design, setting and variables Sample
characteristics
Measure of responsiveness
(type, description)
Summary of results CEBM
level
assigned
Farrow and
Blissett62
Design: prospective cohort.
Setting (country): UK
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
Maternal control in feeding
at 6 months.
Outcome(s): Infant growth
(standardized to reference
adjusting for age and gender)
from birth to 6 months and from
6 to 12 months.
n¼69
Age: 6 months
Age(s) at
follow-up:
12 months
Race/ethnicity: not
reported.
Type: observational
Description of measure: Feeding
Interaction Scale, a coding
system for feeding observations
measuring maternal control
and infant autonomy.
Maternal control moderated infant
weight gain, with infants’ low
control mothers regulating their
growth from birth–12 months
and infants’ high control mothers
having poor growth regulation.
Breast-fed infants had mothers
that were less controlling
than non-breastfed infants.
2b
Li et al.64 Design: -prospective cohort
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
BF intensity (% all milk feedings
that were breastmilk) and bottle–
emptying behaviors (maternal
self-report), at multiple time
points from 1 to 6 months.
Outcome(s): excess infant weight
gain from 7 to 12 month old.
n¼1896
Age: B1 month
Age(s) at follow-
up: 2–12 mon
Race/ethnicity:
83.6% non-
Hispanic White,
6.9% non-Hispanic
Black and 9.5%
Hispanic.
Type: Self-report survey
Description of measure: survey
of infant’s bottle use behavior
(four 5-point Likert scale
questions), focused on bottle
emptying and mother’s
encouragement to finish
a bottle.
High breastfeeding intensity resulted
in significantly reduced excess weight
gain. Infants who often emptied
bottles in the first 6 months of life
were 69% more likely to have excess
weight gain in the second half of
infancy, compared with those who
rarely emptied bottles, but mother’s
self-reported encouragement to
empty bottles was not related to
excess weight gain.
2c
Saxon et al.60 Design: retrospective cohort
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
Infant weight, length, head
circumference at birth, 2, 4- and
6 months.
Outcome(s): mother’s reported
feeding practice from birth to
9 months.
n¼48
Age: 12 months.
Race/Ethnicity:
81% White, 11%
Hispanic, 3% black
and 3% Asian.
Type: Self-report survey
Description of measure: feeding
practices survey (14-items)
focused on demand and
scheduling feeding (e.g., who
(mother or baby) determined
when the baby ate; does
mother recognize fussiness
as a hunger cue).
Feeding style (on-demand versus
scheduled) was not associated with
any growth variables across the first
year of life. However, predicting
growth changes based on current
feeding styles might not capture
the prospective effect of feeding
on growth, as current feeding
behaviors might have changed in
response to infant growth changes.
4
Worobey et al.65 Design: prospective cohort.
Setting (country): US
Independent variable(s)/intervention:
maternal sensitivity to infant-feeding
cues and infant-feeding record
(24-h recall) at 3 and 6 months.
Outcome(s): Infant weight gain
from birth–3 months, 3–6 months
and 6–12 months.
n¼96
Age: newborn
(enrolled at first
WIC visit)
Age(s) at
follow-up: 3, 6
and 12 months.
Race/ethnicity:
76% Hispanic and
24% Black.
Type: observational
Description of measure:
maternal sensitivity to cues
subscale of the Nursing Child
Feeding Assessment Scale.
Infants gained more from 6 to 12
months if their mothers were less
sensitive to satiety cues. Also, infants
whose mother’s reported a greater
number of feeds per day had greater
growth from 6 to 12 months.
Growth between other time points
were not significantly related to the
feeding variables.
3b
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; BSL, baseline; BF, breastfeeding; CEBM, Oxford University Centre for Evidence Based Medicine; RCT, randomized control trial;
UK, United Kingdom; US, United States; WIC, United States Special Nutritional Supplement Program for Women, Infants and Children. aNote: Two studies were
presented within this article, but only the study performed within an infant sample was presented in this review.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
486
International Journal of Obesity
of the original sample completing both the class and the
assessments.
Rybski et al.56 similarly studied the associations between
maternal feeding behaviors and infant intake, in a small
sample of 3-day-old female, white infants. Although the
study observed maternal behaviors related to responsiveness
during bottle feedings (i.e., verbal interaction, eye contact,
tender and caretaking touching), the purpose of the study
was to observe changes in sucking behaviors and intake of
formula across a 24-h period to understand the potential
effects of circadian periodicity. The feeding variables (e.g.,
total feeding time, nutritive sucking time nutritive sucking
count) were measured during six observations set at the same
time for all participants across a 24-h period in a stimuli-free
room, with controlled temperature and lighting. This study
did not find an association between maternal feeding
behaviors and infant energy intake. A potential source of
bias for evaluating feeding responsiveness was that mothers
were not allowed to hold their infants during any feeds,
which could have limited the amount and type of (e.g.,
touching) behaviors a mother was engaged in during a
feeding. In view of these shortcomings of these studies,
additional research is needed to evaluate the effect of
caregiver awareness of infant-feeding cues on infant energy
intake and subsequent weight gain.
Discordant responsiveness and impaired self-regulation. As
shown in Figure 1, we suggest that caregiver feeding
responses that are discordant with infant hunger and
fullness cues could led to impaired satiety response in the
infant. Experimental and observational studies have shown
that infants and toddlers possess an ability to self-regulate
energy intake at and across eating occasions by adjusting
food intake in response to changes in feeding frequency and
the energy content of foods consumed.57–59 Responsive
caregiving is believed to promote cause (i.e., infant cue)
and effect (i.e., caregiver response) learning that is central to
the development of self-regulation.24 Some have suggested
that chronically unresponsive feeding may negatively influ-
ence what children learn about when eating should begin
and end.40,41 This systematic review, however, did not
identify any research that has evaluated the assertion that
feeding interactions affect infant satiation. As such, there is
currently no evidence to suggest that responsiveness influ-
ences child self-regulation of energy intake in infancy or
toddlerhood. The notion that feeding children in the
absence of hunger and continuing to feed beyond fullness
is detrimental to the development of self-regulation has
appeared in scholarly writing for well over half a century.
The findings of this review suggest the need for research
explicitly measuring dimensions of appetite regulation,
including hunger, satiety and satiation. Owing to a lack of
studies and negative finding, evidence that feeding respon-
siveness influences infant self-regulation of energy intake
remains weak.
Discordant responsiveness and accelerated weight gain and
overweight
Compared with other areas of the model, a relatively greater
number of studies were identified involving the association
of responsiveness to infant cues with infant weight status
and weight gain. Saxon et al.60 studied maternal feeding
behaviors and subsequent weight gain. The feeding beha-
viors focused on in Saxon et al.,60 measured whether a
mother reported using demand or schedule feeding in the
first 6 months of life, rather than more directly assessing
responsiveness as we have described. However, the authors
describe demand feeding in the same manner as we have
describe responsive feeding, in which the caregiver would
initiate feeding in response to infant-hunger cues, rather
than based on the external factor of time. Mothers retro-
spectively self-reported their feeding practices from birth to
6 months, and were then classified as demand or schedule
feeders based on the answers to these two questions:
(1) I would classify my feeding philosophy as: (answers:
‘Feeding on demand’ or ‘Feeding on a schedule’) and (2) Who
would you say usually determined your baby’s eating routine?
(answers: ‘Me (myself/caregiver)’ or ‘Baby’). They reported
that feeding style did not significantly predict infant weight
gain at 2, 4 and 6 months (controlling for birth weight).
Although Saxon et al.60 findings do not support a relationship
between maternal feeding style and infant’s future weight
status as proposed in the model, a few shortcomings must be
noted. Foremost, demand versus scheduled feeding does not
explicitly capture ‘responsive feeding’ as we have defined it.
Also breastfeeding was not controlled for during analyses,
even though it has been linked to growth particularly in the
first 2 years of life,61 and the two groups had differential levels
of breastfeeding.60 Lastly, evaluating growth (i.e., change with
time) may have been more informative than assessing weight
at each time point controlling for birth weight.
Farrow and Blissett62 examined a well-researched aspect of
responsive feeding, maternal control and its moderating
effects on infant weight gain in the first year of life. Relative
to the other studies reviewed, the maternal behavior of
interest was well aligned with the definition of ‘responsive
feeding’ used here. Maternal control was measured through
the systematic coding of feeding observations (using the
Feeding Interaction Scale,63 in which the observer rates the
mother on a 1–9 scale, with 1 equaling a very controlling
caregiver (e.g., mother is continuously forcing the infant to
eat) and 9 equaling not controlling (e.g., mother is allowing
the infant autonomy to control his or her own feeding,
although supervising the infant). Infant weight was mea-
sured at 6 and 12 months (birth weight was taken from
hospital records). They found that infant weight gain from
birth to 6 months, and from 6 to 12 months were negatively
correlated, indicating that infants appear to self-regulate
their weight in the first year of lifeFhowever, this was only
found in infants with low maternal control in feeding.
Infants with high-maternal control in feeding had the
opposite patternFweight gain from birth to 6 months and
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
487
International Journal of Obesity
from 6 to 12 months was positively correlated, so that a high
weight gainer from birth to 6 months would continue on the
path of high weight gain. This suggests that, although
infants have the capability of self-regulation of energy intake
and weight gain, there is an interaction with the environ-
ment (caregiver feeding control) that can modify the
expression of this potential. Farrow and Blissett’s62 work
offers insight into the role of one aspect of responsive
feeding (maternal control) and infant growth. The prospec-
tive nature of the study along with rigorous measures helps
to add support to the model proposed here.
In a more recent investigation identified, Li et al.64
reported on a population-based survey of US mothers, which
investigated whether breastfeeding duration and frequency,
and exclusiveness and bottle-emptying practices predicted
excess infant weight gain in the interval between 6 and
12 months. These feeding practices were self-reported at
multiple time points from birth to 6 months. Findings
revealed that infants who often emptied bottles in the first 6
months of life were 69% more likely to have excess weight
gain in the second-half of infancy (6–12 months), when
compared with those who rarely emptied bottles.64 Also,
high breastfeeding intensity (combination of duration and
frequency) resulted in significantly reduced excess weight
gain. This study explores the ‘what’ of infant feeding
(breastmilk versus formula), which is often focused on with
relation to obesity, but also explores the ‘how,’ through the
effects of bottle emptying. These findings highlight
behavioral aspects of infant feeding, which affected infant
weight gain in this large sample (n¼1896). An issue in
interpreting these findings is the extent to which bottle-
emptying behaviors reflected infants’ appetites versus
caregivers’ responsiveness to infant-feeding cues. Additional
research is needed to address this issue.
Worobey et al.65 assessed the association of infant growth
with maternal-feeding attitudes related to responsiveness for
their association with infant growth in a sample of low
income, minority women and their newborn infants.
Maternal ‘pushiness’ during feeding (using the Maternal
Feeding Attitudes Questionnaire66) and maternal sensitivity
to infant cues (using the Nursing Child Assessment Feeding
Scale67,68) were assessed in relation to infant growth from
birth to 12 months. After controlling for numerous factors
(such as birth weight, gender, race/ethnicity, maternal age,
maternal body mass index before pregnancy), infant weight
gain between 6 and 12 months was predicted by mothers’
sensitivity to satiety cues such that low-maternal sensitivity
to infant cues resulted in increased weight gain. It should be
noted that infant growth measures lacked standardization, as
change in actual weight was investigated (i.e., weight gained
from 6 to 12 months of age), no change in growth based on a
standardized growth reference (i.e., weight-for-age Z-score
change). As a result, the changes in growth only speak to
change to one’s own baseline not in reference to whether the
individual’s growth would trail, equal or exceed other
individual’s of similar gender and age.
Two studies were identified that assessed infant obesity/
overweight in relationship with responsive feeding. Dubois
et al.69 sampled two groups of infants; 42 normal weight 4–9
months old (25–75th percentiles and 47 overweight 4–9
months old (o90th percentile). They investigated whether
these two groups differed in energy intake, infant feeding
history and maternal reliance on external cues. For ‘maternal
reliance on external cues’, mothers were asked open-ended
questions and their answers were coded with respect to the
following variables: mother usually uses external cues to
initiate or terminate feedings, sometimes tries to feed more
or less than the infant wants and sometimes offers food to
stop infant’s crying. External cues were described as time or
prepared portion of formula/food and infant signals were
described as sucking fingers (hunger) or turning head away
(fullness). Dubois et al.69 found no differences in maternal
reliance on external cues among the groups of overweight
4- to 9-month-old infants (o90th percentile) and normal
infants (25–75th percentiles). However, the method of
measuring maternal feeding responsiveness was not standar-
dized as open-ended questions were used, which had not
been previously validated. Baughcum et al.70 investigated the
affect of maternal-feeding behaviors on weight in an infant
sample (aged 11–23 months). A separate sample of pre-
schoolers was assessed as part of this study;70 however, those
results are not discussed given the focus of this review on
infants and toddlers. Within the infant sample, a number of
aspects of maternal feeding in which responsiveness is
implicit were assessed for the association with infant weight
status, including concern about the infant’s weight (either
over- or underweight), concern about the infant being
hungry, using food to calm the infant and establishing a
feeding schedule. However, no associations were identified
between a particular feeding style and overweight in infants.
Income was found to influence feeding behaviors, for
example low-income mothers of infants reported more
concern about infant hunger and feeding infant on a
schedule. Thus, the findings of Baughcum et al.70 did not
add support for the model. But a criticism is that the purpose
of the study was in part to validate the questionnaires
usedFthese new questionnaires might have failed to
elucidate responsive feeding behaviors. For example, mater-
nal control in feeding, commonly measured through the
validated Child Feeding Questionnaire,71 has previously
been connected with child overweight,72,73 yet in this study,
control was not associated with child overweight. Also
measuring concurrent weight and feeding behaviors might
not reflect any of the effects from feeding behaviors in
infancy on later childhood overweight. Most of the evidence
was gathered to support the connection between responsive
feeding and weight gain/overweight as presented in the
model, and of the six studies identified, three added support.
Summary of the systematic review findings
The evidence gathered was rated at 2b or lower level of the
CEBM system, meaning there was a dearth of high-quality
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
488
International Journal of Obesity
prospective work. Of the nine studies described here, only
three studies (Farrow and Blissett62; Li et al.64; Worobey
et al.65) revealed associations with dimensions of feeding
responsiveness as described for the model. Although these
studies provide preliminary support for an influence of
responsive feeding on infant/toddler weight, there was a
notable lack of evidence to substantiate the assertion that
such a relationship is produced by excessive energy intake.
Further, the methods for measuring responsive feeding as
reviewed in this narrative and as presented in Table 2,
generally reveal a lack of consistency in the manner in which
feeding responsiveness has been operationally defined. This
concern, along with the failure to consider the bidirectional
nature of maternal responsiveness and infant behaviors,
points to the need for further exploration.
Discussion
This systematic review offered the opportunity to describe
the rationale for investigating responsive feeding and over-
weight during infancy and toddlerhood and to assess the
state of the science on this topic. The conceptual model
provided a framework for identifying and thematically
organizing scientific literature on this topic. The systematic
approach adopted here revealed a dearth of rigorous inquiry
on this topic. The three studies that provided the strongest
support for the model (Farrow and Blissett62; Li et al.64;
Worobey et al.65) evaluated maternal responses to satiety
cues and/or bottle-emptying behaviors and their association
with infant growth. Only Li et al.,64 however, assessed
feeding interactions in a longitudinal manner. In general,
all the articles retrieved, supportive or not, lacked prospec-
tive assessments of infant–caregiver interactions as they
relate to self-regulation, growth and obesity, which was
evidenced by the relatively low CEBM levels assigned to the
studies gathered. This is a crucial gap in the current literature
in light of recognition that feeding interactions are nested in
developmental phases. So that capturing caregiver–infant
interactions at one time point only provides insight into that
point in development. That there was a shortage of support
for the proposed model from the primarily cross-sectional
studies in this systematic review should not discourage
further inquiry.
Beyond the dearth of longitudinal work in this area, the
assessment of the dynamic nature of caregiver–infant
feeding interactions is notably lacking. Responsiveness can
be conceived of as a reciprocal dimension of feeding in
which children are responsible for providing clear feeding
cues and the caregiver for responding in a prompt and
developmentally appropriate manner. However, rather than
focusing on how bidirectional aspects of feeding relate with
obesity outcomes (increased energy intake, rapid weight gain
overweight), studies have focused on how infant-eating
behaviors or caregiver (primarily maternal)-feeding
behaviors independently affect these outcomes. This is
potentially because of the lack of assessment tools, which
measure interactions in a dynamic manner, rather than
assessing the infant or the caregiver alone. To move knowl-
edge ahead in the area of feeding behaviors and obesity
prevention, it is necessary to begin to assess the dyad. Such
assessments would aid in evaluating the usefulness of the
proposed model.
In addition to the alternating focus on either infant or
caregiver behaviors, there was a noteworthy lack of consis-
tency in measurement of ‘responsive feeding.’ The variation
in measurement is illustrated by the variety of constructs
reported on by the articles in this review (Table 2). Variables
used to represent responsive feeding included demand/
schedule feeding,60 bottle emptying,64 observational mea-
sures (e.g., NCAST)56 and self-report measures of caregiver
behaviors.62,70 An additional complicating factor is that
previous research in responsive feeding has been largely
oriented toward undernutrition and has conceptualized
feeding responsiveness more broadly than is considered here
for obesity prevention. Measuring responsive feeding in the
framework of obesity prevention may require a different
operational definition of responsive feeding than those used
in the context of undernutrition. In the model explored
here, discordant responsiveness leads to increases in infant
intake, which could lead to chronic energy imbalance and,
eventually, overweight. The relatively new interest in
viewing responsive feeding in the context of child obesity
risk may explain the lack of concerted study on this topic.
Why has feeding responsiveness been largely ignored in
efforts to understand the development of overweight?
Exploration of feeding dynamics and their role in intake
and growth requires a multidisciplinary perspective invol-
ving parenting, pediatric nutrition and child development.
Psychologists have long studied feeding, but viewed it
primarily as a vehicle for studying parent–child relationships
and developmental outcomes other than growth rather than
as phenomena of interest unto itself. Nutritionists have
historically viewed children’s eating behavior in terms of
food and nutrient intake rather than feeding and eating
behavior. As a result most knowledge related to feeding
pertains to what children are fed rather than how children
are fed. Recognition of environmental contributions to
pediatric overweight and challenges to its treatment have
only recently directed scholarly interest toward understand-
ing familial influences on the development of eating
behavior.
Acknowledging that the caregiver–infant dyad and family
is situated within a broader socioenvironment context
encompassing among others, cultural beliefs/practices about
parenting and feeding, policy, education, healthcare and
childcare, it is clear that a great deal of work remains to be
carried out to begin to understand the complexities of
interactions among these factors and the intrapersonal and
interpersonal factors we think shape dyadic feeding inter-
actions. It is overwhelming and beyond the scope of any
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
489
International Journal of Obesity
single study to undertake such a task, but such a broad
conceptual model as proposed here is useful to the field at
large, pointing to particular relationships that are un- or
underexplored and highlighting areas in which interdisci-
plinary collaborations could be fruitful. As first steps in
advancing knowledge in this area, we offer some suggestions.
The development of well-operationalized and rigorously
developed measurement tools is clearly a priority for moving
scientific inquiry forward in this area. Rigorous observational
approaches should be considered in initial research efforts to
characterize feeding responsiveness and to identify dimen-
sions for which self-report may be possible.74 Qualitative
methods may also prove fruitful to identify potential
facilitators and barriers to responsiveness, such as caregiver
feeding knowledge, attitudes and beliefs. Longitudinal
studies will be required to draw inferences about the capacity
of responsiveness to modify child nutritional and growth
trajectories. Beyond closing in on the caregiver–child inter-
actions, a multidisciplinary approach must be taken to
address the complexity of both caregiver–infant interactions
and obesity and how and whether responsive feeding relates
to the entire growth spectrum (undernutrition to obesity).
Points of exploration, include experiences of the infant and
caregiver during feeding from mood and stress,75,76 altera-
tions in neurohormones77 and racial/ethnic differences and
cultural beliefs, which might alter caregivers’ attitude to
optimal responsiveness, can be investigated.78,79 Thus,
efforts to understand the development of infant/child eating
behavior and its importance for growth should take feeding,
its goals and its context into consideration. The dependence
of infants and toddlers on their caregivers to obtain nutrition
and develop eating skills suggests a potentially critical role
for responsive feeding in nutrition and growth from a very
early point in development. A recent study of overweight
children illustrates the need for such early interventions, as
it found that nearly 60% of overweight children became
overweight before the age of 2 years.3 As a potentially
modifiable behavior, responsiveness may represent an
efficacious target for early obesity prevention efforts in the
future.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the notion that a chronic mismatch between
feeding and child cues contributes to the development of
overweight is not new.29,41 On the basis of the findings of
this systematic review, however, the role of feeding respon-
siveness in accelerated growth and overweight remains, to
date, is more speculative than substantive. There is pre-
liminary support for the proposed role of responsiveness in
growth during early development, though the strength of
evidence is relatively weak and the studies are few. These
findings underscore that we are in the early stage of
empirical research on this topic. As such, we conclude that
additional rigorous investigation of feeding responsiveness is
needed, particularly longitudinal studies, within the frame-
work of early obesity prevention efforts among diverse
populations.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the NIH K01 DK 61319-01
(Fisher), USDA 2005-55215-6 16726 (Johnson), NIH DK
56350 (Hodges), RWJF 66523 (Hodges) and Nestle Infant
Nutrition (Fisher).
References
1 Kral TV, Stunkard AJ, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Brown DD, Faith
MS. Daily food intake in relation to dietary energy density in the
free-living environment: a prospective analysis of children born
at different risk of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 86: 41–47.
2 Stettler N, Iotova V. Early growth patterns and long-term obesity
risk. Curr Opin Clin Nutr 2010; 13: 294–299.
3 Harrington JW, Nguyen VQ, Paulson JF, Garland R, Pasquinelli L,
Lewis D. Identifying the ‘tipping point’ age for overweight
pediatric patients. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2010; 49: 638–643.
4 Stettler N, Kumanyika SK, Katz SH, Zemel BS, Stallings VA. Rapid
weight gain during infancy and obesity in young adulthood in a
cohort of African Americans. Am J Clin Nutr 2003; 77: 1374–1378.
5 Monteiro POA, Victora CG. Rapid growth in infancy and
childhood and obesity in later lifeFa systematic review. Obesity
Rev 2005; 6: 143–154.
6 Wells JC, Chomtho S, Fewtrell MS. Programming of body
composition by early growth and nutrition. Proc Nutr Soc 2007;
66: 423–434.
7 Shonkoff JP, Phillips De. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: the Science
of Early Childhood Development. National Academy Press:
Washington, DC, 2000.
8 Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, Lamb MM, Flegal KM.
Prevalence of high body mass index in US children and
adolescents, 2007–2008. JAMA 2010; 303: 242–249.
9 Scaglioni S, Agostoni C, Notaris RD, Radaelli G, Radice N,
Valenti M. et al. Early macronutrient intake and overweight at
five years of age. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000; 24: 777–781.
10 Mellbin T, Vuille JC. Relationship of weight gain in infancy to
subcutaneous fat and relative weight at 10 1/2 years of age. Br J
Prev Soc Med 1976; 30: 239–243.
11 Mei Z, Grummer-Strawn LM, Scanlon KS. Does overweight in
infancy persist through the preschool years? An analysis of CDC
Pediatric Nutrition Surveillance System data. Soz Praventivmed
2003; 48: 161–167.
12 Stettler N, Zemel BS, Kumanyika S, Stallings VA. Infant weight
gain and childhood overweight status in a multicenter, cohort
study. Pedatrics 2002; 109: 194–199.
13 Reilly JJ, Armstrong J, Dorosty AR, Emmett PM, Ness A, Rogers I
et al. Early life risk factors for obesity in childhood: cohort study.
BMJ 2005; 330: 1357.
14 Dennison BA, Edmunds LS, Stratton HH, Pruzek RM. Rapid infant
weight gain predicts childhood overweight. Obesity 2006; 14:
491–499.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
490
International Journal of Obesity
15 Dubois L, Girard M. Early determinants of overweight at 4.5 years in
a population-based longitudinal study. Int J Obes 2006; 30: 610–617.
16 Karaolis-Danckert N, Buyken AE, Bolzenius K, Perim de Faria C,
Lentze MJ, Kroke A. Rapid growth among term children whose
birth weight was appropriate for gestational age has a longer
lasting effect on body fat percentage than on body mass index.
Am J Clin Nutr 2006; 84: 1449–1455.
17 Blair NJ, Thompson JMD, Black PN, Becroft DMO, Clark PM,
Han DY et al. Risk factors for obesity in 7-year-old European
children: the Auckland Birthweight Collaborative Study. Arch Dis
Child 2007; 92: 866–871.
18 Stettler N, Kumanyika S, Katz SH, Zemel BS, Stallings VA. Rapid
early infancy weight gain and obesity in African American adults.
Circulation 2003; 107: E7002.
19 Stettler N, Stallings VA, Troxel AB, Zhao J, Schinnar R, Nelson SE
et al. Weight gain in the first week of life and overweight in
adulthood: a cohort study of European American subjects fed
infant formula. Circulation 2005; 111: 1897–1903.
20 Monteiro POA, Victora CG, Barros FC, Monteiro LMA. Birth size,
early childhood growth, and adolescent obesity in a Brazilian
birth cohort. Int J Obes 2003; 27: 1274–1282.
21 Ekelund U, Ong K, Linne Y, Neovius M, Brage S, Dunger DB et al.
Upward weight percentile crossing in infancy and early child-
hood independently predicts fat mass in young adults: the
Stockholm Weight Development Study (SWEDES). Am J Clin Nutr
2006; 83: 324–330.
22 Stettler N, Iotova V. Early growth patterns and long-term obesity
risk. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 2010; 13: 294–299.
23 Ong KK, Emmett PM, Noble S, Ness A, Dunger DB, Team AS.
Dietary energy intake at the age of 4 months predicts postnatal
weight gain and childhood body mass index. Pedatrics 2006; 117:
e503–e508.
24 Lamb ME, Easterbrooks MA. Individual differences in parental
sensitivity: origins, components, and consequences. In: Lamb
ME, Sherrod LR, (eds). Infant Social Cognition: Empirical and
Theoretical Considerations. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, 1981; 127–153.
25 Ziv Y, Cassidy J. Maternal responsiveness to infant irritability: the
contribution of Crockenberg and Smith’s ‘Antecedents of
mother-infant interaction and infant irritability in the first
3 months of life’. Infant Behav Dev 2002; 25: 16–20.
26 Feldman R, Greenbaum CW, Yirmiya N. Mother-infant affect
synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control.
Dev Psychol 1999; 35: 223–231.
27 Bronson MB. Self-regulation in Early Childhood: Nature and Nurture.
The Guilford Press: New York, NY, 2000.
28 Green JA, Gustafson GE, Irwin JR, Kalinowski LL, Wood RM.
Infant crying: acoustics, perception, and communication. Early
Dev Parenting 1995; 4: 161–175.
29 Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM. Some Contemporary Patterns of Mother-
infant Interaction in the Feeding Situation Stimulation in Early
Infancy. Academic Press: New York, 1969.
30 Bell SM, Ainsworth MD. Infant crying and maternal responsive-
ness. Child Dev 1972; 43: 1171–1190.
31 Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Maternal responsiveness and
cognitive development in children. New Dir Child Dev 1989;
1989: 49–61.
32 Goldberg S. Social competence in infancy: a model of parent–
infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quart 1977; 23: 163–177.
33 Vallotton CD. Do infants influence their quality of care? Infants’
communicative gestures predict caregivers’ responsiveness. Infant
Behav Dev 2009; 32: 351–365.
34 Wright P, Fawcett J, Crow R. The development of differences in
the feeding behaviour of bottle and breast fed human infants
from birth to two months. Behav Process 1980; 5: 1–20.
35 Wright P. Development of feeding behavior in early infancy:
implications for obesity. Health Bull 1981; 39: 197–205.
36 Fomon SJ, Filer LJ, Thomas LN, Rogers RR, Proksch AM.
Relationship between formula concentration and rate of growth
of normal infants. J Nutr 1969; 98: 241–254.
37 Fomon SJ, Filmer LJ, Thomas LN, Anderson TA, Nelson SE.
Influence of formula concentration on caloric intake and growth
of normal infants. Acta Paediatric Scandinavia 1975; 64: 172–181.
38 Kent JC, Mitoulas LR, Cregan MD, Ramsay DT, Doherty DA,
Hartmann PE. Volume and frequency of breastfeedings and fat
content of breast milk throughout the day. Pedatrics 2006; 117:
e387–e395.
39 Fox MK, Devaney B, Reidy K, Razafindrakoto C, Ziegler P.
Relationship between portion size and energy intake among
infants and toddlers: evidence of self-regulation. J Am Diet Assoc
2006; 106 (1S): 77–83.
40 Wright P. Learning experiences in feeding behaviour during
infancy. J Psychosom Res 1988; 32: 613–619.
41 Costanzo PR, Woody EZ. Domain-specific parenting styles and
their impact on the child’s development of a particular deviance:
the example of obesity proneness. J Soc Clin Psychol 1985; 4:
425–445.
42 Bruch H. Eating Disorders: Obesity, Anorexia Nervosa, and the Person
Within. Basic Books: New York, 1973.
43 Birch LL, Fisher JO. Development of eating behaviors among
children and adolescents. Pediatrics 1998; 101: 539–549.
44 Engle PL, Bentley ME, Pelto G. The role of care in nutrition
programmes:current research a research agenda. Proc Nutr Soc
2000; 59: 25–35.
45 Bornstein MH. Sensitive periods in development: structural
characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychol Bull 1989;
105: 179–197.
46 Dewey K. Guiding principles for complementary feeding of the breastfed
child. PAHO/WHO: Washington, DC, 2003. http://www.who.int/
nutrition/publications/guiding_principles_compfeeding_breastfed .
47 Bronfenbrenner U. Ecology of the family as a context for human
development: research perspectives. Dev Psychol 1986; 22: 723–742.
48 Taveras EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-
Shiman SL. Racial/ethnic differences in early-life risk factors for
childhood obesity. Pediatrics 2010; 125: 686–695.
49 World Health Organization U. Global Strategy for Infant and Young
Child Feeding. WHO: Geneva, 2003.
50 Burdette HL, Whitaker RC, Hall WC, Daniels SR. Maternal infant-
feeding style and children’s adiposity at 5 years of age. Arch
Pediatr Adolesc Med 2006; 160: 513–520.
51 Susin LR, Giugliani ER, Kummer SC, Maciel M, Simon C,
da Silveira LC. Does parental breastfeeding knowledge increase
breastfeeding rates? Birth 1999; 26: 149–156.
52 Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine OU. Levels of evidence and
grades of recommendation. Available at: http://www.cebm.net/
levels_of_evidence.asp. (Accessed 9 January 2009) 2009.
53 Mogan J. Parental weight and its relation to infant feeding
patterns and infant obesity. Int J Nurs Stud 1986; 23: 255–264.
54 Francis LA, Birch LL. Maternal weight status modulates the effects
of restriction on daughters’ eating and weight. Int J Obes Relat
Metab Disord 2005; 29: 942–949.
55 Kavanagh KF, Cohen RJ, Heinig MJ, Dewey KG. Educational
intervention to modify bottle-feeding behaviors among formula-
feeding mothers in the WIC program: impact on infant formula
intake and weight gain. J Nutr Educ Behav 2008; 40: 244–250.
56 Rybski DA, Almli CR, Gisel EG, Powers J, Maurer Jr M. Sucking
behaviors of normal 3-day-old female neonates during a 24-h
period. Dev Psychobiol 1984; 17: 79–86.
57 Birch LL, Deysher M. Caloric compensation and sensory specific
satiety: Evidence for self regulation of food intake by young
children. Appetite 1986; 7: 323–331.
58 Birch LL, Deysher M. Conditioned and unconditioned caloric
compensation: evidence for self-regulation of food intake by
young children. Learn Motiv 1985; 16: 341–355.
59 Fomon S. Nutrition of Normal Infants. Mosby-Year Books: St Louis,
Missouri, 1993.
60 Saxon TF, Gollapalli A, Mitchell MW, Stanko S. Demand feeding
or schedule feeding: infant growth from birth to 6 months.
J Reprod Infant Psychol 2002; 20: 89–100.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
491
International Journal of Obesity
http://www.cebm.net/levels_of_evidence.asp
http://www.cebm.net/levels_of_evidence.asp
61 Kalies H, Heinrich J, Borte N, Schaaf B, von Berg A, von Kries R
et al. The effect of breastfeeding on weight gain in infants: results
of a birth cohort study. Eur J Med Res 2005; 10: 36–42.
62 Farrow C, Blissett J. Does maternal control during feeding
moderate early infant weight gain? Pediatrics 2006; 118: e293–e298.
63 Wolke D, Skuse D. The management of infant feeding problems.
In: Cooper P, Stein A (eds). Feeding Problems and Eating Disorders in
Children and Adolescents. Harwood: Chur, Switzerland, 1992; 27–
59.
64 Li R, Fein SB, Grummer-Strawn LM. Association of breastfeeding
intensity and bottle-emptying behaviors at early infancy with
infants’ risk for excess weight at late infancy. Pediatrics 2008;
122 (Suppl 2): S77–S84.
65 Worobey J, Lopez MI, Hoffman DJ. Maternal behavior and infant
weight gain in the first year. J Nutr Educ Behav 2009; 41: 169–175.
66 Kramer MS, Barr RG, Leduc DG, Boisjoly C, Pless IB. Maternal
psychological determinants of infant obesity. Development and
testing of two new instruments. J Chronic Dis 1983; 36: 329–335.
67 Barnard K. Caregiver/Parent-child Interaction Feeding Manual. Uni-
versity of Washington School of Nursing: Seattle, WA, 1994.
NCAST Publications.
68 Hodges EA, Houck GM, Kindermann T. Reliability of the Nursing
Child Assessment Feeding Scale during toddlerhood. Issues Compr
Pediatr Nurs 2007; 30: 109–130.
69 Dubois S, Hill DE, Beaton GH. An examination of factors believed
to be associated with infantile obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 1979; 32:
1997–2004.
70 Baughcum AE, Powers SW, Johnson SB, Chamberlin LA, Deeks
CM, Jain A et al. Maternal feeding practices and beliefs and their
relationship to overweight in early childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr
2001; 22: 391–408.
71 Birch LL, Fisher JO, Castro CN, Grimm-Thomas K, Sawyer R,
Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding
Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and
practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite
2001; 36: 201–210.
72 Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent-child
feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and
weight status. Obes Res 2004; 12: 1711–1722.
73 Spruijt-Metz D, Lindquist CH, Birch LL, Fisher JO, Goran MI.
Relation between mothers’ child-feeding practices and children’s
adiposity. Am J Clin Nutr 2002; 75: 581–586.
74 Sacco LM, Bentley ME, Carby-Shields K, Borja JB, Goldman BD.
Assessment of infant feeding styles among low-income African-
American mothers: comparing reported and observed behaviors.
Appetite 2007; 49: 131–140.
75 Groer MW. Differences between exclusive breastfeeders, formula-
feeders, and controls: a study of stress, mood, and endocrine
variables. Biol Res Nurs 2005; 7: 106–117.
76 Farrow CV, Blissett JM. Is maternal psychopathology related
to obesigenic feeding practices at 1 year? Obes Res 2005; 13:
1999–2005.
77 Swain JE, Lorberbaum JP, Kose S, Strathearn L. Brain basis of early
parent-infant interactions: psychology, physiology, and in vivo
functional neuroimaging studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007;
48: 262–287.
78 Kumanyika SK. Environmental influences on childhood obesity:
ethnic and cultural influences in context. Physiol Behav 2008; 94:
61–70.
79 Taveras EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rich-Edwards JW,
Rifas-Shiman SL. Racial/ethnic differences in early-life risk factors
for childhood obesity. Pediatrics 2010; 125: 686–695.
Responsive feeding and infant overweight
KI DiSantis et al
492
International Journal of Obesity
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: a systematic review
Introduction
Overweight and accelerated weight gain in infancy and toddlerhood
Caregiving and self-regulation among infants and toddlers
A potential role for responsive feeding
Materials and methods
Literature-search strategy
Figure 1 Figure 1 presents a model for the pathway from discordant feeding responsiveness to accelerated weight gain and/or overweight.
Study selection and grading
Results
Systematic review results
Table 1 Systematic review search results by search term
Narrative review
Socioenvironmental context of discordant responsiveness
Discordant responsiveness and feeding frequency and amount
Figure 2 Flow diagram of article extraction.
Table 2 Systematic review results
Outline placeholder
Discordant responsiveness and impaired self-regulation
Discordant responsiveness and accelerated weight gain and overweight
Summary of the systematic review findings
Discussion
Conclusion
Conflict of interest
Acknowledgements
References
_______________________________________________________________
_______________________________________________________________
Report Information from ProQuest
February 10 2015 14:37
_______________________________________________________________
10 February 2015 ProQuest
Table of contents
1.
…………………………………………………………………………… 1
10 February 2015 ii ProQuest
Document 1 of 1
Child-care usage and mother-infant “quality time”
Author: Booth, Cathryn L; Clarke-Stewart, K Alison; Deborah Lowe Vandell; McCartney, Kathleen; Margaret
Tresch Owen
ProQuest document link
Abstract: Mothers’ time-use paterns were compared in families in which infants spent more than 30 hours per
week in child care (In-Care group; n = 143) versus 0 hours per week (At-Home group; n = 183) from birth to 6
months of age. In-Care group mothers spend about 12 fewere hours per week interacting with their infants, for
about 32% less time; fathers of these infants were more involved in caregiving.
Full text: Headnote
Mothers’ time-use patterns were compared in families in which infants spent more than 30 hours per week in
child care (In-Care group; n = 143) versus 0 hours per week (At-Home group; n = 183) from birth to 6 months of
age. In-Care group mothers spent about 12 fewer hours per week interacting with their infants, for about 32%
less time; fathers of these infants were more involved in caregiving. The groups did not differ in the quality of
mother-infant interaction. In the In-Care group, quantity of in
Headnote
teraction was related to greater separation anxiety and concerns about effects of maternal employment. Time-
use data were not related to child outcomes at 15 months of age. Results suggest that the effect of extensive
time spent apart on the quantity and quality of mother-infant interaction may be smaller than anticipated.
Headnote
Key Words: child care, mother-infant interaction, time use.
Dramatic increases in the labor force participation of women with young children in the United States have led
to a concomitant increase in the use of early and extensive nonmaternal child care, usually beginning in the
early months of life (Hofferth, Brayfield, Deich, &Holcomb, 1991). The increasing use of extensive child care in
early infancy has generated scientific debate about the effects of child care on children’s development and the
mother-child relationship (see Lamb, 1998). One of the chief concerns is that early entry into child care may
have a negative impact on the ability of the mother and infant to develop a synchronous relationship. That is,
long hours spent apart may provide insufficient time for the mother to learn about her infant’s cues and
biological rhythms (Brazelton, 1986), and her appropriate and sensitive responses to these cues are vital for
fostering the infant’s optimal development (see Kelly &Barnard, 2000).
A particular area of concern is the infant’s attachment security to the mother. According to attachment theory,
the degree of mother-infant sensitivity and responsivity forms the basis for the development of the infant’s
secure (or insecure) attachment to her (see De Wolff &van Ijzendoorn, 1997). The security of this attachment, in
turn, is important because it predicts children’s subsequent peer relations, affect regulation, selfesteem, ego
resiliency, and behavior problems (see Thompson, 1998). Thus, to the extent that maternal sensitivity and
responsivity decrease when the mother and infant spend long hours apart, then we would expect an increase in
the risk of insecure attachment and other less optimal outcomes.
Infants form important and independent attachments to their fathers and other caregivers as well. However, a
comparison of the relative influence of infant-mother and infant-father attachment on child outcomes has
indicated that the infant-mother relationship is a stronger predictor (Thompson, 1998). These results, coupled
with concerns about the effects on children of large increases in maternal employment hours, have led to a
focus on the early mother-child, rather than father-child, relationship within the child-care and employment
literature.
Within the child-care literature, several studies focusing on early infancy have supported the hypothesis of a
10 February 2015 Page 1 of 12 ProQuest
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/219764964?accountid=11330
negative association between the amount of child care and the quality of motherinfant interaction, but the
majority of researchers have not (see National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child
Care Research Network [NICHD ECCRN], 1999, for a review). Analyses from the NICHD Study of Early Child
Care are especially relevant in this regard, because a subset of these data were used in the present report.
Longitudinal analyses of the main data set indicated that greater hours per week in child care were related to
somewhat less maternal sensitivity and child engagement in mother-child interaction in the first 3 years of life
(NICHD ECCRN, 1999). However, this relatively small decrease in the quality of mother-child interaction did not
appear to have an impact on the infants’ attachment security to their mothers, except when mothers were highly
insensitive (NICHD ECCRN, 1997).
In all of these studies, the quality of motherchild interaction and attachment security have been evaluated in
relation to the number of hours that the child spends in child care or the number of hours that the mother is
employed. However, it is significant that none of them has assessed the amount of time that the mother and
child actually spend together, and in the absence of empirical evidence, it is generally assumed that hours of
maternal employment serve as a proxy for the reduction in the amount of time spent in motherchild interaction
(Baydar, Greek, &Gritz, 1999). Contrary to this assumption, evidence from studies based on time-use diaries
and interviews has shown that maternal employment is related to a relatively small reduction in social
interaction and a larger reduction in physical care (Baydar et al., 1999; Hilton, 1990; Nock &Kingston, 1988;
Walker &Woods, 1976), with differences of less than 5 hours of care per week between employed and
nonemployed mothers (Douthitt, 1989; Sanik, 1990).
One limitation of these time-use studies is the assumption that time spent in mother-child interaction is, by
definition, “quality time” that will enhance children’s development (Bryant, 1992). However, large individual
differences exist in the quality of interaction when mothers and children do spend time together, and these
differences predict child outcomes in cognitive, linguistic, and socioemotional domains (e.g., Baumrind, 1989;
NICHD ECCRN, in press). Thus, it is important to measure directly the quality and quantity of mother-child
interaction.
It is also likely that there are individual differences in the extent to which mothers, regardless of employment
hours, optimize the time that is available for interacting with their infants. In some families, employed mothers
may make special efforts to spend high-quality time with their infants during nonwork hours or on the weekends,
whereas in other families, the stresses of multiple roles and responsibilities may leave mothers with very little
time or energy to devote to their infants when they are at home together (Hochschild, 1989; Rachlin, 1987).
Similarly, it is likely that some stay-at-home mothers focus on spending a lot of high-quality interaction time with
their children, whereas others are mostly present in the home while engaging in other primary activities (Baydar
et al., 1999; Bianchi, 2000; Nock &Kingston, 1988).
Some of the variance in the amount of time mothers and infants spend together may be due, in part, to
demographic factors. Maternal education, for example, is positively related to amount (e.g., Bryant &Zick, 1996;
Hill &Stafford, 1980) and quality (e.g., Clarke-Stewart, Gruber, &Fitzgerald, 1994; NICHD ECCRN, 1999) of
motherchild social interaction.
Another source of individual differences in the time mothers spend with their infants may be the mothers’
psychological characteristics. For example, maternal separation anxiety may positively affect the extent to which
employed mothers are motivated to spend time with their infants when they are not at work. In support of this
hypothesis, Bradley, Whiteside-Mansell, Brisby, and Caldwell (1997) found that greater maternal separation
anxiety was related to mothers’ having greater socioemotional investment in their children. Also, Fagan (1994)
found that among employed mothers, greater maternal separation anxiety was positively related to time spent
with their infants in on-site child-care settings.
Attitudes about employment also may affect the extent to which mothers spend quality time with their infants.
Mothers who believe that the consequences of maternal employment for children are relatively negative are less
10 February 2015 Page 2 of 12 ProQuest
likely to be employed and are less likely to view maternal employment as acceptable at younger child ages
(Greenberger, Goldberg, Crawford, &Granger, 1988). If employed mothers must spend long hours away from
their children despite negative attitudes about employment consequences, we would expect these mothers to
maximize the amount of time they spend with their children when they have the opportunity to be together.
Nock and Kingston (1988) have suggested that fathers may compensate for the decreased time spent by
employed mothers with their children, thereby reducing the potential negative effects of maternal employment
on child outcomes. However, both Sanik (1990) and Douthitt (1989) found that fathers’ hours of caring for their
children did not differ as a function of maternal employment, even though these hours have increased in the
past few decades. Although the present study focuses primarily on the mother-infant relationship, we also
sought to determine whether fathers’ involvement with their infants varied according to the time these infants
spent apart from their mothers, and the effects of both father involvement and mother-infant time on child
outcomes.
The following questions were addressed in the present study: Among families in which the study infants spent
more than 30 hours per week in child care, versus those in which child care was not used, (a) How do the
mothers compare in the amount of time spent in instrumental care and social interaction with their infants? (b)
How do the mothers compare in the quality of interaction? (c) Is the quality of mother-infant interaction related to
the amount of time they spend together? (d) Do mothers’ psychological characteristics and fathers’ involvement
differ by group, and are they related to the amount of time mothers spend with their infants? and (e) Do the
quantity and quality of maternal care and father involvement predict child outcomes at age 15 months?
METHOD
Participants
Data were analyzed from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care, a prospective longitudinal study of the effects of
child care on children’s development. Participants were recruited during the first 11 months of 1991 from
hospitals at 10 research sites that were located in or near Little Rock, Arkansas; Irvine, California; Lawrence,
Kansas; Boston, Massachusetts; Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; Charlottesville, Virginia;
Morganton, North Carolina; Seattle, Washington; and Madison, Wisconsin. During selected sampling periods,
all women giving birth in each hospital were screened. Mothers were excluded if they were giving the baby up
for adoption, had a known or acknowledged history of substance abuse, were under 18 years of age, did not
speak English, planned to move in the next year, or lived outside the area or in a neighborhood considered
unsafe for visits; mothers were also excluded if the baby was part of a multiple birth, was not discharged within
7 days of birth, or had a congenital disability (see NICHD ECCRN, 1999, for more details).
Of the 1,364 participating mothers, 1,171 completed at least one time-use interview when their infants were 7
months of age. However, the sample for the present paper was further reduced to 326 because it included only
(a) mothers who were married or partnered and lived in the same household as the spouse or partner; (b)
mothers who completed the time-use interview on two occasions-one weekday and one weekend day (and, if
the mother was employed, one of these days had to be a work day and the other a nonwork day); (c) mothers of
infants spending 0 hours in nonmaternal child care from birth to 6 months of age (At-Home group; n = 183) or
mothers of infants spending an average of 30 or more hours per week in child care from birth to 6 months (In-
Care group; n = 143). Fathers also participated in the study at 6 of the 10 sites. Among the 326 families, data
were available for 126 fathers (64 in the At-Home group and 62 in the In-Care group).
Demographic characteristics of the At-Home group, the In-Care group, and the remaining families whose data
were not included (n = 845) are shown in Table 1. Compared with the nonincluded mothers, the mothers in the
present report were significantly older (29.27 vs. 28.07 years), t(1,169) = 3.32, p = .001. The groups did not
differ in parental education, family income-to– needs ratio, number of children, child gender, or ethnicity.
None of the mothers in the At-Home group was employed or in school from birth through 6 months, and none of
the children in this group spent any time in nonmaternal child care. Most of the In-Care group mothers (n = 135)
10 February 2015 Page 3 of 12 ProQuest
were employed or in school at least 30 hours per week (M = 40.42 10.34 hours), and their children were in child
care for a mean of 36.01 ( 4.78) hours per week.
Demographic Variables
When the infants were 1 month old, mothers were interviewed at home to obtain information about the mother’s
age and parents’ education; the infant’s gender, birth order, and ethnicity. The family income-to-needs ratio was
obtained at 6 months. The income-to-needs ratio is an index of family economic resources, with higher scores
indicating greater financial resources per person in the household. Family income (exclusive of welfare
payments) was divided by the poverty threshold, which was based on total family size and number of children
under 18 years of age.
Psychological Characteristics of Mothers
The mothers’ psychological characteristics were assessed during home visits when the infants were 1 and 6
months of age.
Maternal separation anxiety. The Maternal Separation Anxiety Scale is a 21-item questionnaire adapted from
Hock, Gnezda, and McBride (1983). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The items assess the mother’s sadness, worry, and guilt about separation from
her infant; beliefs about the importance of maternal care and her infant’s preference for maternal care; and
beliefs about her infant’s ability to adapt to nonmaternal care. Cronbach’s alphas were .93 at both 1 and 6
months. An average score was computed across ages.
Attitudes about maternal employment. At the 1month home visit the mothers completed 11 items of the Beliefs
About the Consequences of Maternal Employment for Children (Greenberger et al., 1988). The scale, which
uses 6-point Likert ratings (1 = disagree very strongly to 6 = agree very strongly), yields Benefits and Costs
scores. A high score on the Costs subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = .88) reflects the belief that children are likely to
have problems if their mothers are employed (e.g., “Children are less likely to form a warm and secure
relationship with a mother who is working full time”).
Father Involvement in Caregiving
When the infants were 6 months old, fathers at 6 of the 10 study sites completed a set of questionnaires,
including one describing their caregiving activities with the study child (NICHD ECCRN, 2000). The 11
caregiving activities (which included items such as bathing, feeding, reading to the infant, attending to the infant
when crying) were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = partner’s job, 3 = we share equally, 5 = my job) and summed to
yield a total score for Father Involvement in Caregiving (Cronbach’s alpha = .77).
Quantity of Mother-Infant Interaction
Quantity of mother-infant interaction was assessed when the infants were 7 months old, using a telephone time-
use interview with the mother on two occasions-one describing the immediately prior weekday-workday
(Monday through Thursday) and the other, the prior weekend-nonwork day (Saturday or Sunday).
The interview is a chronological account of what the mother did during the entire 24-hour period beginning at 1
minute after midnight on the previous day. The mother identified (a) her primary activity, (b) activity beginning
and end times, (c) her location, (d) the infant’s location, and (e) any secondary activity. Activity codes included
Caring for Child (instrumental care, interaction, supervision or discipline, helping or teaching, organized outings,
10 February 2015 Page 4 of 12 ProQuest
outdoor play, other child care), Paid Work, Household Activity, Travel, Personal Care, Education/Training,
Organizational Activities, Social Activity, Quiet Leisure.
The activity codes yielded two summary variables, computed separately for weekdays and weekends:
Instrumental Care consisted of the total amount of time per day that the mother engaged in instrumental care,
supervision or discipline, and other child care with the study child as either primary or secondary activities.
Social Interaction consisted of the total amount of time per day that the mother spent interacting, helping, or
teaching, and being on organized outings with the study child as primary or secondary activities.
The validity of short-term recall methods of time-use data collection has been demonstrated via comparison with
a variety of other methods (Juster &Stafford, 1985; see Robinson, 1999). Within the NICHD data set, validity
data were obtained at one of the sites by use of the Experience Sampling Method (Larson &Csikszentmihalyi,
1983) at 7 months of age for 48 families who were called 60 times over 4 weeks. The percentage of calls in
which the mother was interacting with the child correlated at r = .56 with the total amount of mother-child
interaction (instrumental care plus social interaction) assessed via the time-use calls.
Quality of Interaction
Sensitivity in the HOME. The Infant/Toddler Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME;
Caldwell &Bradley, 1984) was administered during the 6-month home visit. The HOME is a semistructured
interview/observational procedure in which a home visitor answers a set of binary questions based upon
maternal response to specific queries and makes observations of materials in the home and the mother’s
behavior toward the child. Factor analysis of the items on the HOME yielded two relevant factor scores, Positive
Involvement and Lack of Negativity. The former scale comprised six items assessing the extent to which the
mother was positively responsive and affectionate to her infant during the home visit. The latter scale comprised
five items measuring the extent to which the mother refrained from behaving in a negative manner toward her
infant (e.g., shouting, spanking, annoyance, restriction) during the visit. The two scale scores were summed to
produce a composite score for Sensitivity in the HOME.
Child Outcomes
The specific child outcomes were selected to broadly represent important areas of development at 15 months-
cognition, language, social engagement, and attachment security.
Bayley Mental Development Index. The Bayley Mental Development Index (MDI) (Bayley, 1969) is the most
widely used measure of cognitive developmental status for children in the first 2 years of life. The assessment
measures sensory perceptual acuity and discrimination; memory, learning, and problem solving; early verbal
communication; and the ability to form generalizations and classifications. The MDI was administered in a
laboratory playroom setting at 15 months of age by trained examiners.
MacArthur Communication Development Inventory. The Communication Development Inventory (CDI) (Fenson,
Dale, Reznick, Thai, &Reilly, 1991) assesses early language development via a 493-item inventory that was
completed by the mothers when their children were 15 months old. The infant version of the CDI includes a 396-
item vocabulary checklist on which the mother indicates which words her child uses and which words her child
understands. Separate percentile scores for vocabulary production and vocabulary comprehension were
computed.
10 February 2015 Page 5 of 12 ProQuest
10 February 2015 Page 6 of 12 ProQuest
10 February 2015 Page 7 of 12 ProQuest
10 February 2015 Page 8 of 12 ProQuest
Child engagement. During a home visit, mothers were asked to have their infants play with ageappropriate toys
in three containers in a set order for 15 minutes. Mother-infant interaction was videotaped, and the tapes from
all sites were shipped to a central location for coding. A series of 4-point ratings was used to characterize the
behaviors of the mothers and infants separately. Two of the ratings of child behavior–child engagement and
positive mood-were summed to yield a child engagement composite. Cronbach’s alpha for the composite at 15
months was .58, and reliability was .74.
CONCLUSION
In summary, our results indicate that among mothers whose infants were in 30 or more hours per week of child
care, the “cost” in terms of reduced mother-infant interaction time was considerably less than expected, and the
quality of mother-infant interaction did not appear to be affected by the amount of time spent apart. It is likely
that these mothers attempted to compensate for time spent apart by focusing increased attention on their
infants during the weekend, and this was especially true if the mother was concerned about her separation from
the infant and the effects of her employment on the child. Fathers, as well, may have attempted to compensate
for maternal employment hours by being more involved with their infants.
Compared with prior maternal time-use studies, our investigation focused on a number of unique areas.
Namely, we addressed psychological predictors of the quantity of time mothers spent interacting with their
infants, the relation between quantity and quality of interaction, and the consequences for the infant’s
development of the quantity and quality of maternal care. Additionally, we identified the importance of
considering weekday and weekend time separately.
It is clear that the data in this report have provided a first step toward expanding the study of maternal time use,
but it is also clear that the results have raised additional questions that can be answered only in the context of
longitudinal in-depth research that includes more extensive time-use data and that includes both parents.
Perhaps the most important of these questions is the extent to which the quantity of parent-child interaction is
related to child outcomes over time and at older ages. Although time-use researchers have tended to assume
that parents who spend a lot of time with their infants are providing high-quality parenting that will promote
optimal development, our results suggest the need for careful consideration of the complex relations between
the quantity of interaction, the quality of parenting, and child outcomes.
Footnote
NOTE
Footnote
The authors are investigators in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and acknowledge the generous support of
their time by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (Grants HD25447, HD25456,
HD27040, HD25451). The study is directed by a steering committee and supported by NICHD through a
cooperative agreement (UlO) that calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff.
The authors would like to thank the coinvestigators who worked with them on the design of the larger study, the
site coordinators and research assistants who collected the data, and the families and teachers who continue to
participate in this longitudinal study.
References
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., &Wall,
S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological
References
study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow
(pp. 349-378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Baydar, N., Greek, A., &Gritz, R. M. (1999). Young mothers’ time spent at work and time spent caring for
10 February 2015 Page 9 of 12 ProQuest
children. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 20, 61-84.
References
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Bianchi, S. M. (2000, March). Maternal employment and time with children: Dramatic change or surprising
continuity? Presidential address at the meeting of the Population Association of America, Los Angeles, CA.
Bradley, R. H., Whiteside-Mansell, L., Brisby, J. A., &Caldwell, B. M. (1997). Parents’ socioemotional investment
in children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 59, 77-90.
References
Brazelton, T B. (1986). Issues for working parents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 14-25.
Bryant, W. K. (1992). Human capital, time use, and other family behavior. Journal of Family and Economic
Issues, 13, 395-405.
Bryant, W. K., &Zick, C. D. (1996). An examination of parent-child shared time. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 58, 227-237.
Caldwell, B. M., &Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock:
University of Arkansas at Little Rock.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Gruber, C. P., &Fitzgerald, L. M. (1994). Children at home and in day care. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
De Wolff, M. S., &van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental
antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571-591.
Douthitt, R. A. (1989). The division of labor within the home: Have gender roles changed’? Sex Roles, 20, 693-
704.
References
Fagan, J. (1994). Correlates of maternal involvement in on-site and off-site day care centers. Child and Youth
Care Forum, 23, 275-290.
Fenson, L., Dale, P S., Reznick, J. S., Thai, D., &Reilly, J. S. (1991). Technical manual for MacArthur
Communicative Development Inventories. San Diego, CA: San Diego State University.
Greenberger, E., Goldberg, W., Crawford, T J., &Granger, J. (1988). Beliefs about the consequences of
maternal employment for children. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12, 35-59.
Hill, R. C., &Stafford, E P (1980). Parental care of children: Time diary estimates of quantity, predictability, and
variety. Journal of Human Resources, 15, 219-239.
References
Hilton, J. M. (1990). Differences in allocation of family time spent on household tasks among single-parent, one-
earner, and two-earner families. Lifestyles: Family and Economic Issues, 11, 283-298.
Hochschild, A. (1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home. New York: Viking. Hock,
E., Gnezda, M. T, &McBride, S. L. (1983).
Mothers of infants: Attitudes toward employment and
References
motherhood following birth of the first child. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 425-431.
Hofferth, S. L., Brayfield, A., Deich, S., &Holcomb, P (1991). National child care survey, 1990. Washington, DC:
Urban Institute Press.
Juster, F T, &Stafford, F P (Eds.) (1985). Time, goods, and well-being. Ann Arbor, MI: Survey Research Center,
Institute for Social Research.
Kelly, J. E, &Barnard, K. E. (2000). Assessment of parent-child interaction: Implications for early intervention. In
J. P Shonkoff &S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp. 258289). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Lamb, M. E. (1998). Nonparental child care: Context, quality, correlates. In W. Damon, I. E. Sigel, &K. A.
10 February 2015 Page 10 of 12 ProQuest
Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, 5th ed., pp. 73-134). New York: Wiley.
Larson, R., &Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. New Directions for Methodology of
Social and Behavioral Science, 15, 4156.
References
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment
security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68, 860-879.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1999). Child care and mother-child interaction in the first three
years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399-1413.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2000). Factors associated with fathers’ caregiving activities and
sensitivity with young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 200-219.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (in press). Parenting and family influences when children are in
child care: Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. In J. G. Borkowski, S. Ramey, &M. Bristol-Power
(Eds.), Parenting and the child’s world: Influences on intellectual, academic, and social-emotional development.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Nock, S., &Kingston, P W. (1988). The division of leisure and work. Social Science Quarterly, 70, 2439.
References
Rachlin, V. C. (1987). Fair vs. equal role relations in dual-career and dual-earner families: Implications for family
interventions. Journal of Applied Family and Child Studies, 36, 187-192.
Robinson, J. P (1999). The time-diary method: Structure and uses. In W. E. Pentland, A. S. Harvey, M. P
Lawton, &M. McColl (Eds.), Time use research in the social sciences (pp. 47-89). New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Sanik, M. M. (1990). Parents’ time use: A 1967-1986 comparison. Lifestyles, 11, 299-316.
Thompson, R. A. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In W. Damon &N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of
child psychology (Vol. 3, 5th ed., pp. 25104). New York: Wiley.
Walker, K. E., &Woods, M. E. (1976). Time use: A measure of household production of family goods and
services. Washington, DC: Center for the Family of the American Home Economics Association.
Weinraub, M., Jaeger, E., &Hoffman, L. (1988). Predicting infant outcome in families of employed and
nonemployed mothers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 3, 361-378.
AuthorAffiliation
CATHERINE L. BOOTH University of Washington
K. ALSION CLARKE-STEWART University of California-Irvine*
DEBORAH LOWE VANDELL University of Wisconsin**
KATHLEEN MCCARTNEY Harvard University***
MARGARET TRESCH OWEN University of Texas-Dallas****
AuthorAffiliation
Department of Family &Child Nursing, University of Washington, CHDD 106 South Building, Box 357920,
Seattle, WA 98195-7920 (ibcb@u.washington.edu).
AuthorAffiliation
*Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California-Irvine, 3340 Social Ecology 11, Irvine,
CA 92697-7085.
**Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin, 1025 West Johnson Street, Room 467,
Madison, WI 53706.
AuthorAffiliation
***Harvard Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, 704 Larsen Hall, Appian Way, Cambridge, MA
02138.
****School of Human Development, University of Texas– Dallas, P 0. Box 830688, Mail Station GR 41,
10 February 2015 Page 11 of 12 ProQuest
Richardson, TX 75083-0688.
Subject: Comparative studies; Mothers; Babies; Child care;
Publication title: Journal of Marriage and Family
Volume: 64
Issue: 1
Pages: 16-26
Number of pages: 11
Publication year: 2002
Publication date: Feb 2002
Year: 2002
Publisher: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
Place of publication: Minneapolis
Country of publication: United Kingdom
Publication subject: Public Health And Safety, Matrimony, Education, Sociology, Children And Youth – About
Source type: Scholarly Journals
Language of publication: English
Document type: Feature
ProQuest document ID: 219764964
Document URL: http://nclive.org/cgi-
bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/219764964?accountid=11330
Copyright: Copyright National Council on Family Relations Feb 2002
Last updated: 2014-06-21
Database: ProQuest Central
_______________________________________________________________
Contact ProQuest
Copyright 2015 ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. – Terms and Conditions
10 February 2015 Page 12 of 12 ProQuest
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/219764964?accountid=11330
http://nclive.org/cgi-bin/nclsm?url=http://search.proquest.com/docview/219764964?accountid=11330
http://www.proquest.com/go/contactsupport
http://search.proquest.com/info/termsAndConditions
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The impact of infant crying on the parent-infant relationship: The …
Oldbury, Sarah, RGN, SCPHN, MSc PHNP;Adams, Karen, RN, RM, RHV, RNT, Pgdip HPE, M
Community Practitioner; Mar 2015; 88, 3; ProQuest Central
pg. 29
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Delivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.
You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.
Read moreEach paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.
Read moreThanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.
Read moreYour email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.
Read moreBy sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.
Read more